Published on in Vol 9 (2025)

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/65732, first published .
Fluctuations in Daily Happiness and Nervousness Based on Depressive and Anxious Symptoms in Adolescents or Young Adults Across 3 Latin American Cities: Experience Sampling Study

Fluctuations in Daily Happiness and Nervousness Based on Depressive and Anxious Symptoms in Adolescents or Young Adults Across 3 Latin American Cities: Experience Sampling Study

Fluctuations in Daily Happiness and Nervousness Based on Depressive and Anxious Symptoms in Adolescents or Young Adults Across 3 Latin American Cities: Experience Sampling Study

1CRONICAS Centre of Excellence in Chronic Diseases, Universidad Peruana Cayetano Heredia, Lima, Peru

2Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry, East London NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom

3Department of Biomedical Informatics, School of Medicine, University of Utah, Utah, United States

4Instituto Peruano de Orientación Psicológica, Lima, Peru

5Department of Health and Social Sciences, University of the West of England (UWE), Bristol, United Kingdom

6Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia

7Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary University of London, 58 Turner Street, London, United Kingdom

8Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, School of Medicine, University of Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina

9Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia

10Hospital Universitario San Ignacio, Bogotá, Colombia

Corresponding Author:

Catherine Fung, MA


Background: Experience sampling methods (ESMs) have been used in clinical research to collect data on emotional and behavioral states in real-life contexts among different populations. Although the use of ESMs in mental health has increased, it has not been applied to larger samples of young people in disadvantaged urban settings.

Objective: This study aimed to determine the extent to which mood status scores (happiness and nervousness) vary during a week, as a function of having or not having symptoms of depression or anxiety, in a sample of adolescents and young adults in the cities of Buenos Aires, Bogotá, and Lima. A secondary objective was to identify factors associated with mood scores, including sociodemographics, quality of life, and daily activities.

Methods: This study was part of the Building Resilience and Resources to Reduce Mental Distress in Young People in Latin America research program, which focuses on mental health resources for young people. Participants (n=143) aged 15‐24 years completed daily ESM assessments over a week using the mobile app, resulting in 5246 reports. Data were analyzed using descriptive analyses with 2-tailed t tests and chi-square tests, and multilevel linear regression was used to examine associations between depressive or anxiety symptoms, mood variability, and factors influencing mean mood. Finally, Spearman correlation assessed the relationship between happiness and nervousness.

Results: The analysis revealed that depressive or anxiety symptoms were not significantly associated with increased variability in mood scores (happiness P=.40 and nervousness P=.84). However, males exhibited greater variability in happiness and nervousness scores (P<.001) than females. Additionally, young people showed higher variability in nervousness than adolescents (P=.02). Regarding average happiness scores, young adults reported higher average happiness than adolescents (β=.604; P=.003). Engaging in structured activities (eg, sports, music lessons, and dance classes) was associated with increased happiness (β=.266; P=.01). In contrast, instrumental activities (eg, cleaning, shopping, meal preparation, or taking medication; β=−.144; P=.02) and work-related tasks (β=−.205; P=.01) were linked to lower happiness and higher nervousness (β=.387; P<.001). Quality of life was positively correlated with happiness (β=.486; P<.001) and negatively correlated with nervousness (β=−.273; P=.005). Finally, as for average scores, a strong negative correlation was found between happiness and nervousness (rs=−0.92; P<.001). The simple multilevel analysis showed that for each point of happiness, nervousness decreased by 0.45 points (95% CI −0.48 to −0.42; t3=−41.7; P<.001; SE 0.01).

Conclusions: Our study reveals that depressive and anxiety symptoms do not significantly affect the variability in predicted happiness and nervousness scores. However, we observed that demographic factors, such as gender and age, play a role in emotional variability.

JMIR Form Res 2025;9:e65732

doi:10.2196/65732

Keywords



Background

Adolescence and young adulthood are crucial periods of social, behavioral, and psychological development and change [Roach A, Sureshkumar DS, Elliot K, et al. One-year recovery rates for young people with depression and/or anxiety not receiving treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. Jul 21, 2023;13(7):e072093. [CrossRef] [Medline]1]. However, these periods are also associated with increased vulnerability to mental health problems, with most functional mental disorders typically manifesting before the age of 25 years [Solmi M, Radua J, Olivola M, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry. Jan 2022;27(1):281-295. [CrossRef] [Medline]2]. According to the World Health Organization, approximately 14% of young people worldwide experience mental health problems, with anxiety and depression being the most prevalent [Mental health of adolescents. World Health Organization. URL: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health [Accessed 2025-05-14] 3]. These conditions contribute to approximately 13% of the total health burden in this age group [Biswas T, Scott JG, Munir K, et al. Global variation in the prevalence of suicidal ideation, anxiety and their correlates among adolescents: a population based study of 82 countries. EClinicalMedicine. Jul 2020;24:100395. [CrossRef] [Medline]4], and they are more likely to experience depression and anxiety than older adults [Sikström S, Kelmendi B, Persson N. Assessment of depression and anxiety in young and old with a question-based computational language approach. Npj Ment Health Res. Jul 24, 2023;2(1):11. [CrossRef] [Medline]5]. As a result, the World Health Organization has identified the reduction of depression and anxiety in adolescents as a key priority, particularly in low- and middle-income countries [Zhao H, Li L, Zhang X, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of depressive disorders among young people aged 10-24 years, 2010-2019. J Psychiatr Res. Feb 2024;170:47-57. [CrossRef] [Medline]6,Santomauro DF, Herrera AMM, Shadid J. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet. Nov 6, 2021;398(10312):1700-1712. [CrossRef] [Medline]7]. This includes Latin America, where adolescents and young people living in large cities are more exposed to risk factors for developing anxiety and depression [Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. Jun 2012;38(4):661-671. [CrossRef] [Medline]8,Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK. Depression in adolescence. Lancet. Mar 17, 2012;379(9820):1056-1067. [CrossRef] [Medline]9]. Some studies in Latin America have estimated that the prevalence of depression or anxiety among young people varies, such as 17% in Colombia [Gómez-Restrepo C, Bohórquez A, Tamayo Martínez N, et al. Trastornos depresivos y de ansiedad y factores asociados en la población de adolescentes colombianos, Encuesta Nacional de Salud Mental 2015. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría. Dec 2016;45:50-57. [CrossRef]10], 16% in Peru [Y 75 años después una generación optimista lista para transformar el perú y el mundo. UNICEF Perú. URL: https:/​/www.​unicef.org/​peru/​informe/​75-anos-despues-generacion-optimista-lista-transformar-peru-mundo [Accessed 2025-05-14] 11], and 20% in Argentina [Ballarino F. ¿La juventud sin preocupaciones? los problemas de salud mental en la adolescencia UNICEF | para cada infancia. UNICEF Argentina. 2022. URL: https:/​/www.​unicef.org/​argentina/​historias/​la-juventud-sin-preocupaciones-los-problemas-de-salud-mental-en-la-adolescencia [Accessed 2025-05-14] 12].

Evidence suggests that researchers and health professionals should join forces to implement integrated actions to understand the behavior of the incidence and prevalence of mental health problems in young people as one of the first steps in the prevention chain [Colizzi M, Lasalvia A, Ruggeri M. Prevention and early intervention in youth mental health: is it time for a multidisciplinary and trans-diagnostic model for care? Int J Ment Health Syst. 2020;14(1):23. [CrossRef] [Medline]13]. This should complement retrospective data analysis, which is vulnerable to attrition and recall bias [Tzouvara V, Kupdere P, Wilson K, Matthews L, Simpson A, Foye U. Adverse childhood experiences, mental health, and social functioning: a scoping review of the literature. Child Abuse Negl. May 2023;139:106092. [CrossRef] [Medline]14], with techniques that explore the triggering of symptoms, considering contextual and social factors and elements of everyday life. In this sense, collecting data on people’s experiences and mental states in their daily lives is proposed from psychological and social science research.

The experience sampling method (ESM) is a daily technique of brief assessments that allows data collection in daily life [Verhagen SJW, Hasmi L, Drukker M, van Os J, Delespaul P. Use of the experience sampling method in the context of clinical trials. Evid Based Ment Health. Aug 2016;19(3):86-89. [CrossRef] [Medline]15]. The ESM uses repeated self-reports through a short questionnaire that participants complete when they receive a signal (eg, phone notifications). The reports assess participants’ experiences and behavior in real time, in their natural environment, and at multiple time points throughout their daily lives [Verhagen SJW, Hasmi L, Drukker M, van Os J, Delespaul P. Use of the experience sampling method in the context of clinical trials. Evid Based Ment Health. Aug 2016;19(3):86-89. [CrossRef] [Medline]15]. The ESM has several methodological advantages: (1) it improves accuracy and ecological validity, (2) it provides an assessment of participants in their natural environment in real time and at multiple points, and (3) it offers a large and dynamic variation in data analysis and reporting [Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1-32. [CrossRef] [Medline]16-Oorschot M, Kwapil T, Delespaul P, Myin-Germeys I. Momentary assessment research in psychosis. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(4):498-505. [CrossRef]18]. Statistically, ESM studies typically generate substantial data, as they can record multiple measures per day over several days. Consequently, ESM data can be used at a descriptive level (eg, estimating the average of a specific variable) as well as at a more complex level (eg, examining variability in the relationships between variables across different groups) [Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, et al. Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. Jun 2018;17(2):123-132. [CrossRef] [Medline]19].

The ESM has been used in clinical research as a tool for a range of psychological, psychophysiological, cognitive, and behavioral data in real-life contexts in diverse populations [Oorschot M, Kwapil T, Delespaul P, Myin-Germeys I. Momentary assessment research in psychosis. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(4):498-505. [CrossRef]18,Kim H, Kim S, Kong SS, Jeong YR, Kim H, Kim N. Possible application of ecological momentary assessment to older adults’ daily depressive mood: integrative literature review. JMIR Ment Health. Jun 2, 2020;7(6):e13247. [CrossRef] [Medline]20,Kivelä L, van der Does WAJ, Riese H, Antypa N. Don’t miss the moment: a systematic review of ecological momentary assessment in suicide research. Front Digit Health. 2022;4:876595. [CrossRef] [Medline]21], including adolescents and young people [Mölsä ME, Lax M, Korhonen J, Gumpel TP, Söderberg P. The experience sampling method in monitoring social interactions among children and adolescents in school: a systematic literature review. Front Psychol. 2022;13:844698. [CrossRef] [Medline]22,Li YM, Hachenberger J, Lemola S. The role of the context of physical activity for its association with affective well-being: an experience sampling study in young adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Aug 23, 2022;19(17):10468. [CrossRef] [Medline]23]. Compared with traditional techniques (eg, cross-sectional or longitudinal studies), ESM methodology limits recall bias and captures individual variation in mental health indicators in everyday life [Oorschot M, Kwapil T, Delespaul P, Myin-Germeys I. Momentary assessment research in psychosis. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(4):498-505. [CrossRef]18,Aldao A. The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. Mar 2013;8(2):155-172. [CrossRef] [Medline]24]. Multiple measures of an individual using the ESM help to explore affective variability in more detail and identify the context in which adaptive emotional regulation is used [Aldao A. The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. Mar 2013;8(2):155-172. [CrossRef] [Medline]24].

Mood status variability is the changes and fluctuations in how we feel emotionally over time [Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]25]. High emotional variability can disrupt an individual’s well-being and trigger emotion dysregulation [Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]25]. In this way, studies frequently focus on the role of basic emotions (eg, happiness, nervousness or anxiety, anger, and sadness) because of their relationship to well-being or mental health problems [Neumann A, van Lier PAC, Frijns T, Meeus W, Koot HM. Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent psychopathology: a one-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. Jul 2011;39(5):657-669. [CrossRef] [Medline]26]. This variability tends to occur to a greater extent during adolescence [Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]25]. Capturing these variabilities may aid in the development of future assessments of mental health symptoms. Indeed, longitudinal studies have highlighted the importance of mood variability, given that high levels of “negative” emotions (ie, emotional inertia) may contribute to anxiety and depressive symptoms [Neumann A, van Lier PAC, Frijns T, Meeus W, Koot HM. Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent psychopathology: a one-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. Jul 2011;39(5):657-669. [CrossRef] [Medline]26,Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F. Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. Dec 2010;99(6):1042-1060. [CrossRef] [Medline]27]. Research has also found that mood variability is associated with differences in support from others; for example, adolescents reported higher levels of negative mood when they perceived less support from their parents [Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, et al. Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. Jun 2018;17(2):123-132. [CrossRef] [Medline]19].

Moreover, applying this methodology to adolescents highlights the association between mental health indicators and the natural context through the average daily scores. Some studies have reported that adolescents with higher levels of negative mood (eg, depressive symptoms, stress, and negative affect) spend more time alone and at home and have reduced physical activity. Other studies have reported that physical activity [Kim H, Kim S, Kong SS, Jeong YR, Kim H, Kim N. Possible application of ecological momentary assessment to older adults’ daily depressive mood: integrative literature review. JMIR Ment Health. Jun 2, 2020;7(6):e13247. [CrossRef] [Medline]20,Aldao A. The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. Mar 2013;8(2):155-172. [CrossRef] [Medline]24,Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]25], engaging in artistic activities, and being outdoors are associated with higher levels of affective well-being [Li YM, Hachenberger J, Lemola S. The role of the context of physical activity for its association with affective well-being: an experience sampling study in young adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Aug 23, 2022;19(17):10468. [CrossRef] [Medline]23,Holt NJ. Using the experience-sampling method to examine the psychological mechanisms by which participatory art improves wellbeing. Perspect Public Health. Jan 2018;138(1):55-65. [CrossRef] [Medline]28-Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]30].

Objective

Although the use of the ESM in mental health is becoming more widespread, it has not been applied to larger samples of adolescents and young adults in deprived urban areas of Latin America. Therefore, this study used the ESM to determine the extent to which mood status scores (happiness and nervous) vary during a week, as a function of having or not having symptoms of depression or anxiety, in a sample of adolescents and young adults in the cities of Buenos Aires (Argentina), Bogotá (Colombia), and Lima (Peru). A secondary aim was to identify factors associated with mood scores, such as sociodemographics, wellness factors (quality of life), and daily activities.


Study Design

This study is part of the building resilience and resources to reduce mental distress in young people in Latin America (OLA) research program, designed to identify the personal and social resources that help young people prevent and recover from mental distress [Priebe S, Fung C, Brusco LI, et al. Which resources help young people to prevent and overcome mental distress in deprived urban areas in Latin America? A protocol for a prospective cohort study. BMJ Open. Sep 13, 2021;11(9):e052339. [CrossRef] [Medline]31]. We conducted an ESM study in 3 Latin American countries. This study was designed according to the CREMAS (Adapted Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Checklist for Reporting Ecological Momentary Assessments Studies) checklist [Liao Y, Skelton K, Dunton G, Bruening M. A systematic review of methods and procedures used in ecological momentary assessments of diet and physical activity research in youth: an adapted STROBE Checklist for Reporting EMA Studies (CREMAS). J Med Internet Res. Jun 21, 2016;18(6):e151. [CrossRef] [Medline]32], which is an adapted STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) Checklist for Reporting Ecological Momentary Assessments.

Ethical Considerations

This study’s protocol and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidad de Buenos Aires (10-02-2020), Pontificia Universidad Javeriana (FM-CIE-1138‐20), and Universidad Peruana Cayetano Heredia (Constancia 581-33-20). Furthermore, it received approval from the Research Ethics Committee of Queen Mary University of London (QMERC2020/02).

In line with ethical guidelines, all young adult participants completed an informed consent form, adolescents completed an informed assent, and their parent or legal guardian provided informed consent. As for the consent procedure, participants were informed about the nature of this study before their participation. They were provided with a detailed explanation of the activity, and those who were willing to participate gave oral or written consent, depending on the specific requirements of this study.

Regarding data privacy and confidentiality, all study data were collected anonymously. No personally identifiable information was linked to the data, and steps were taken to ensure that participants’ confidentiality was maintained throughout the research process.

Compensation for participation varied by country. In Peru, participants were offered a voucher redeemable at a chain shop, valued at 50 soles (at an approximate exchange rate of PEN 1=US $0.27). In Colombia, participants received a voucher worth 140,000 Colombian pesos (COP 1=US $0.00021), while in Argentina, compensation was given in the form of cash worth 2000 Argentine pesos (ARS 1=US $0.0053). The compensation was provided to thank participants for their time and involvement in this study.

Participants

The sample consisted of participants between adolescents (15 and 16 y) and young adults (20 to 24 y) from a cohort of the OLA research program from Buenos Aires, Bogotá, and Lima [Priebe S, Fung C, Brusco LI, et al. Which resources help young people to prevent and overcome mental distress in deprived urban areas in Latin America? A protocol for a prospective cohort study. BMJ Open. Sep 13, 2021;11(9):e052339. [CrossRef] [Medline]31]. This research program focused on key developmental stages. This study included 2 groups; the first group comprised individuals aged 15‐16 years, as mental health issues often emerge during this period [Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JRZ, et al. Depression from childhood into late adolescence: influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. J Abnorm Psychol. Nov 2015;124(4):803-816. [CrossRef] [Medline]33]. The second group included young adults aged 20‐24 years, a stage following adolescence but still relevant for the recurrence of mental health disorders [Benjet C, Albor YC, Bocanegra ES, et al. Incidence and recurrence of depression from adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal follow-up of the Mexican Adolescent Mental Health Survey. J Affect Disord. Feb 15, 2020;263:540-546. [CrossRef] [Medline]34]. Participants aged 17‐19 years were deliberately excluded to concentrate on the distinct stages of early adolescence and young adulthood, both of which provide valuable insights into mental health outcomes.

The recruitment of OLA targeted areas within the poorest 50% of neighborhoods, as identified using the Human Development Index [Human Development Index. Human development reports. United Nations. URL: https://hdr.undp.org/data-center/human-development-index [Accessed 2025-05-27] 35] in Bogotá and Lima, and the Unsatisfied Basic Needs Index in Buenos Aires [Santos ME. Measuring Multidimensional Poverty in Latin America: Previous Experience and the Way Forward. Oxford Poverty & Human Development Initiative; 2013. ISBN: 978-1-907194-53-536]. This approach aimed to ensure that the sample was representative of disadvantaged populations in these urban areas. Additionally, the recruitment was carried out through collaborations with service care, community organizations, schools, and youth groups in each of the cities.

This study excluded people with severe mental health problems (such as psychosis, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia), cognitive impairments, illiteracy, or who were users of Huawei (Huawei Technologies Co, Ltd) phones due to issues that arose during the ESM testing phase. Additionally, participants were excluded if they reported a gender other than male or female and if they did not complete the data on the scales used.

Research assistants initially contacted 359 potential participants in the OLA program, primarily via phone calls and messaging. Of these, 7.8% (28/359) were not interested in participating, 10% (36/359) could not be contacted, 19% (68/359) were excluded due to using Huawei phones, 7.5% (27/359) did not complete the informed consent process, and 13.1% (47/359) did not complete training on app usage.

The average time between participation in the OLA cohort baseline questionnaire and enrollment in this study was 107.6 (SD 29.7) days. For further details, please refer to the main publication regarding this activity [Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]30].

Procedures

Participants who met the inclusion criteria were invited to participate in this study, and informed consent or assent was obtained from those who agreed. Following consent, participants attended a briefing session where the eMoodie (eMoodie Ltd) app was installed on their phones. During this session, they were trained to use the app to complete the Experience Sampling Questionnaire (ESQ).

The eMoodie app, designed specifically for youths, facilitates experience sampling techniques by delivering notifications at random intervals [Domoff SE, Banga CA, Borgen AL, et al. Use of passive sensing to quantify adolescent mobile device usage: feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary validation of the eMoodie application. Human Behav and Emerg Tech. Jan 2021;3(1):63-74. URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/25781863/3/1 [Accessed 2025-05-14] [CrossRef]37]. Over 7 days, participants received notifications to complete ESQs. Notifications for participants aged 15‐16 years were scheduled 5 times daily on school days (excluding school hours) and 8 times daily on weekends. For participants aged 20‐24 years, notifications were sent 8 times daily throughout the 7 days [Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]30]. Their availability was considered to adjust notification times.

Notifications were sent as pop-up alerts on participants’ phones, accompanied by a sound alert. Each notification prompted participants to complete an ESQ, which included questions to assess their current mood. Follow-up was conducted throughout this study to ensure compliance. If participants did not respond to notifications during the first half of the day (morning or afternoon), a follow-up call was made to check their progress. If they had not completed the ESQs on day 2, they were contacted again the next day. Additionally, a follow-up call was made if participants completed less than 50% of the ESQs on days 1 and 2. Participants who had responded to more than 50% of the notifications on day 2 were not contacted. Additionally, support was provided as needed, including app troubleshooting and rescheduling of notifications. No extensions were given to participants who missed notifications, as the study adhered to a strict 7-day timeline.

The final dataset was cleaned to remove invalid responses, such as duplicates or ESQs completed in less than 1 second, indicating “skipping,” and the study protocol specified that only participants who completed at least 40% of the notifications (compliance rate) were included in the final dataset [Priebe S, Fung C, Brusco LI, et al. Which resources help young people to prevent and overcome mental distress in deprived urban areas in Latin America? A protocol for a prospective cohort study. BMJ Open. Sep 13, 2021;11(9):e052339. [CrossRef] [Medline]31]. After data cleaning, the mean compliance rate was 76%, ranging from 34% to 100%.

Instruments

Basal Measurements

As part of the OLA cross-sectional study [Gómez-Restrepo C, Diez-Canseco F, Brusco LI, et al. Mental distress among youths in low-income urban areas in South America. JAMA Netw Open. Mar 3, 2025;8(3):e250122. [CrossRef] [Medline]38], all participants completed a questionnaire (online or paper) that included the following measures.

The Patient Health Questionnaire-8 (PHQ-8) was used to measure the severity of depressive symptoms. Eight items are on a 4-point Likert scale (0=not at all, 1=several days, 2=more than half of the days, and 3=almost every day). Participants were considered to have depressive symptoms if they had a cutoff score of 10 or more [Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. Apr 2009;114(1-3):163-173. [CrossRef] [Medline]39].

The General Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) was used to measure the severity of anxiety symptoms. It has 7 items on a 4-point Likert scale (0=not at all, 1=several days, 2=more than half of the days, and 3=almost every day). Participants were considered to have anxiety symptoms if they had a cutoff score of 10 or more [Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Monahan PO, Löwe B. Anxiety disorders in primary care: prevalence, impairment, comorbidity, and detection. Ann Intern Med. Mar 6, 2007;146(5):317-325. [CrossRef] [Medline]40].

Our study categorized participants into 2 groups: those with depressive or anxious symptoms and those without symptoms. They were considered to have depressive or anxious symptoms if their scores on the PHQ-8 or GAD-7 were 10 points or more [Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. Apr 2009;114(1-3):163-173. [CrossRef] [Medline]39,Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Monahan PO, Löwe B. Anxiety disorders in primary care: prevalence, impairment, comorbidity, and detection. Ann Intern Med. Mar 6, 2007;146(5):317-325. [CrossRef] [Medline]40], respectively.

The Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA) was used to measure satisfaction with life, work or unemployment, financial situation, number and quality of friendships, sex life, leisure activities, housing, personal safety, people living with others or living alone, family relationships, physical health, and mental health. Satisfaction is measured using a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1=totally dissatisfied to 7=totally satisfied [Priebe S, Huxley P, Knight S, Evans S. Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA). Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1999;45(1):7-12. [CrossRef] [Medline]41].

As for sociodemographic characteristics, information was collected on participants’ age, gender (male or female), country (Argentina, Colombia, or Peru), main occupation (work, study, housewife, other, or no current occupation), if currently working (no or yes), and education level (primary and high school education, or technical and university education).

ESM Component

The following ESQ questionnaire [Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]30] consisted of the following questions:

  1. “What are you doing right now?” for a free description of the main activity being performed. Responses were open text in Spanish and were first organized into simple codes for activities such as walking, eating, bathing, etc. The codes were then grouped into 6 broad categories based on categories identified in the literature: (1) Basic activities: activities related to self-maintenance and self-care. Some examples are eating, dressing, personal care and grooming, mobility, etc [Edemekong PF, Bomgaars DL, Sukumaran S, Schoo C. Activities of daily living. In: StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing; 2024. [Medline]42]. (2) Instrumental activities: these are more complex than previous ones and allow people to adapt to their environment. Examples include managing finances, cleaning the home, shopping, preparing meals, taking medication, etc [Edemekong PF, Bomgaars DL, Sukumaran S, Schoo C. Activities of daily living. In: StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing; 2024. [Medline]42]. (3) Relaxation activities: this category refers to more informal and unstructured activities that young people engage in to relax, unwind, or simply pass the time without a specific purpose. This can include activities such as watching television, listening to music, chatting with friends, playing video games, reading for pleasure, taking a nap, etc [Badura P, Hamrik Z, Dierckens M, et al. After the bell: adolescents’ organised leisure-time activities and well-being in the context of social and socioeconomic inequalities. J Epidemiol Community Health. Jul 2021;75(7):628-636. [CrossRef] [Medline]43]. (4) Structured activities: these are organized, planned, and have a specific purpose. Generally, these activities are scheduled in advance and have a set timetable, such as sports practice, music lessons, dance lessons, academic tutoring, etc [Mesurado B. Actividad estructurada vs. actividad desestructurada, realizadas en solitario vs. en compañía de otros y la experiencia óptima. Anales de Psicología. 2009;25(2):308-315. URL: https://revistas.um.es/analesps/article/view/87711/84501 [Accessed 2025-06-06] 44]. The latter type of activity included (5) work and (6) educational activities.
  2. “How happy or sad do you feel right now?” answered with a 7-point scale from 1=very happy to 7=very sad.
  3. “How nervous or calm are you right now?” answered with a 7-point scale from 1=very nervous to 7=very relaxed and calm). In this case, we reverse the scores so that higher scores represent higher nervousness levels.

Statistical Analyses

Characteristics of the Participants

A descriptive analysis was conducted based on the sociodemographic characteristics and variables of interest of the participants. The analysis was performed for the total sample and stratified by the presence or absence of depressive or anxious symptoms. For numerical variables, a 2-tailed Student t test was used to assess differences in sociodemographic characteristics based on the presence of depressive or anxious symptoms. Additionally, for categorical variables, a chi-square test was performed to evaluate the association between depressive or anxious symptoms with categorical variables.

Average Levels of Happiness and Nervousness

Data were reviewed to identify and remove invalid observations and duplicates. The average compliance rate after cleaning was 76% [Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]30]. A multilevel linear regression analysis was conducted to examine the association between having depressive or anxious symptoms (dichotomized exposition 0 and 1) and the dimensions of happiness (ranging from 1 to 7) and nervousness (ranging from 1 to 7) as criterion variables. We used restricted maximum likelihood. We considered a composite exposure to be the presence of any conditions for the presence of depressive or anxiety symptoms. This analysis considered the nested structure of the data. The first level represented each measurement of happiness and nervousness, averaging 5 to 8 measurements daily. The second level represented each day, with a total of 7 days. Finally, the third level corresponded to each participant, 143 participants (approximately 50 for each country). We consider the presence of depressive or anxious symptoms, gender, age group, quality of life, educational level, and whether the participant is currently working as fixed effects. Additionally, we consider the happiness and nervousness scores as random effects, so we consider these variables using a random intercept and fixed slopes, allowing each participant to have their baseline level of both variables. Further, our analysis controls the time between self-report measures, as we expect measures of happiness and nervousness taken closer together in time to be more correlated than those taken further apart [Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. Nov 1996;53(11):1033-1039. [CrossRef] [Medline]45].

The use of a multilevel linear regression model accounted for the variability both between measurements taken on each day and between participants, allowing us to capture the hierarchical nature of the data and to examine the association of depressive and anxious symptoms with the dimensions of happiness and nervousness, also adjusting for relevant sociodemographic factors. The effect size was estimated using the adjusted coefficient and a 95% CI. Statistical significance was assessed at a significance level of less than .05. A random effects model was used to account for the nested structure of the data.

Variability in Predicted Happiness and Nervousness Scores (Main Analysis)

Our study assessed the variance of the predicted scores for happiness and nervousness using the previous multilevel linear analysis results. We examined whether there were significant differences in the variances of these predicted scores between different groups, such as gender and age groups, to determine whether the variances were equal or differed significantly between them.

Our ANOVA focuses on a robust test for equality of variances. Significant values mean that the variances of the tested groups are different (P<.05), and therefore, the scores of 1 group have more variability than another. The Levene test analysis assumes that the scores are independent [Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. Nov 1996;53(11):1033-1039. [CrossRef] [Medline]45]. Although our data are nested, we considered it appropriate to use this test as it was applied to the predicted values of happiness and nervousness obtained from the linear multilevel model. These predictions were adjusted for covariates, including both the random and fixed effects of the model. This consideration ensures that the predictions adequately reflect variations in the data adjusted for these covariates.

Relationship Between Happiness and Nervousness

We conducted a correlation analysis using the predicted scores obtained from the full multilevel model to assess the correlation between these 2 variables. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov normality test with Lilliefors correction identified that the normal distribution assumption was unmet. We performed a correlation analysis between predicted happiness and nervousness scores using Spearman correlation coefficient because a nonnormal distribution was assumed, as these are categorical ordinal values. We used the cutoff of the weak (>0.2), moderate (>0.5), and strong correlations (>0.8).

Additionally, our study conducted a simple multilevel analysis to explore the relationship between happiness and nervousness, assuming the same standards as previously mentioned. This analysis adopts a model incorporating a fixed slope, random intercept, and random effects for happiness and nervousness. We do not use any covariates.


Characteristics of the Participants

Initially, 151 individuals participated in the original study; however, 3 participants were excluded for reporting different genders, 4 participants were excluded for not providing MANSA data, and 1 participant was excluded for not reporting education level. Therefore, this study’s sample consisted of 143 participants from Argentina (n=48, 33.6%), Colombia (n=46, 32.2%), and Peru (n=49, 34.3%). The mean age of the participants was 18.6 (SD 3.4) years.

The participants were reported over 7 days, and a total of 5246 reports were collected or completed. Regarding the activities performed by the participants, relaxation activities were the most common (n=1904, 36.3%), followed by basic activities (n=1033, 19.7%). Most participants were women (n=85, 59.4%), exhibited depressive or anxious symptoms (n=86, 60.1%), had studied as their primary occupation (n=105, 73.4%), and were not current smokers (n=104, 72.7%). These activities and the sociodemographic characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics and participant activities in adolescents and young adults across 3 Latin American cities (N=143).
Overall (n=143)With depressive or anxious symptoms (n=86)Without depressive or anxious symptoms (n=57)P value
Numerical variable, mean (SD)a
Age (years)18.6 (3.4)18.7 (3.5)18.5 (3.3).75
MANSAb score4.65 (1.1)4.22 (1)5.26 (0.9)<.001
Happiness (weekly average)4.96 (1.4)4.78 (1.5)5.24 (1.3)<.001
Nervousness (weekly average)2.95 (1.6)3.17 (1.6)2.64 (1.4)<.001
Categorical variable, n (%)c
Gender.32
Male58 (40.6)32 (37.2)26 (45.6)
Female85 (59.4)54 (62.8)31 (54.4)
Country.98
Argentina48 (33.6)29 (33.7)19 (33.3)
Colombia46 (32.2)28 (32.6)18 (31.6)
Peru49 (34.3)29 (33.7)20 (35.1)
Age group.81
Adolescents (15 and 16 y)71 (49.7)42 (48.8)29 (50.9)
Young adults (20 to 24 y)72 (50.3)44 (51.2)28 (49.1)
Main occupation.96
Work19 (13.3)12 (14)7 (12.3)
Study105 (73.4)62 (72.1)43 (75.4)
Housewife7 (4.9)5 (5.8)2 (3.5)
Other3 (2.1)2 (2.3)1 (1.8)
No current occupation9 (6.3)5 (5.8)4 (7)
Currently working.55
No104 (72.7)25 (29.1)14 (24.6)
Yes39 (27.3)61 (70.9)43 (75.4)
Education level.70
Primary and high school education98 (68.5)60 (69.8)38 (66.7)
Technical and university education45 (31.5)26 (30.2)19 (33.3)
Activities carried outd
Basic activities1033 (19.7)588 (18.9)445 (20.8).09
Relaxation activities1904 (36.3)1170 (37.6)734 (34.3).01
Educational activities557 (10.6)284 (9.1)273 (12.8)<.001
Structured activities129 (2.5)45 (1.5)84 (3.9)<.001
Instrumental activities884 (16.8)547 (17.6)337 (15.8).08
Work activities309 (5.9)215 (6.9)94 (4.4)<.001

a Student t test

bMANSA: Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life.

cChi-square test.

dAs the denominator, 5246 evaluations were performed, and only the most relevant activities were reported.

Participants obtained a mean score of 5.05 (SD 1.57) on the happiness scale, which is closer to being happy than to being sad. As for the mean score for nervousness, it was 4.96 (SD 1.42), and this mean score leans more toward nervousness than calmness. Table 1 presents the differences in sociodemographic characteristics between participants with and without depressive or anxious symptoms. Participants with these symptoms showed lower quality of life and happiness scores and higher nervousness scores.

Variability in Predicted Happiness and Nervousness Scores

According to our multilevel model, having depressive or anxiety symptoms did not result in significant differences in the variability of predicted happiness (P=.40) and nervousness (P=.84) scores compared to not having symptoms. This means that participants with depressive or anxious symptoms do not show greater variability in their scores between the different measurements; that is, there are no large changes from high to low scores from one measurement to another compared to people without such symptoms.

However, we did observe a relationship between the variations in the predicted nervousness scores and some demographic variables, including gender and age group. Specifically, males and individuals aged 20 to 24 years exhibited higher variability in their nervousness scores than females and adolescents, respectively.

Similarly, differences in the variations of the predicted happiness scores were associated with gender. Thus, male participants exhibited the highest variability in their happiness scores (Table 2).

Table 2. Variability in happiness and nervousness scores in adolescents and young adults across 3 Latin American cities. Our analysis used a robust test for equality of variances. Analysis was performed on 143 participants with a total of 5246 experience sampling method reports.
HappinessNervousness
SDW50P valueSDW50P value
Gender107.3<.00126<.001
Female0.4740.373
Male0.5610.408
Age group0.4.515.2.02
Adolescents (15 and 16 y)0.5240.397
Young adults (20 to 24 y)0.5390.420
Depressive or anxious symptoms0.7.400.84
No0.4920.354
Yes0.5050.342
Day0.6.700.5.77
Monday0.5390.418
Tuesday0.5360.418
Wednesday0.5420.418
Thursday0.5250.402
Friday0.5270.409
Saturday0.5270.401
Sunday0.5550.421

Average Mean Scores of Happiness and Nervousness

Young adults had higher mean scores of happiness than adolescents (β=.604; P=.003; 95% CI 0.214 to 0.994). In addition, quality of life scores were associated with higher happiness scores (β=.486; P<.001; 95% CI 0.334 to 0.637). Regarding specific activities they engaged in during the assessment, participants who were engaged in structured activities had higher mean happiness scores than those who did not engage in these activities (β=.266; P=.01; 95% CI 0.055 to 0.476). On the contrary, participants who were engaged in instrumental activities (β=−.144; P=.02; 95% CI −0.263 to −0.026) or work activities (β=−.205; P=.01; 95% CI −0.362 to −0.048) had lower mean happiness scores than those who were not engaged in these activities (Table 3).

The mean scores of nervousness were negatively associated with quality of life scores (β=−.273; P=.005; 95% CI −0.463 to −0.084) and were higher among those engaged in work activities (β=.387; P<.001; 95% CI 0.214 to 0.561) compared to those who did not (Table 4).

Table 3. Average happiness levels in adolescents and young adults across 3 Latin American cities. Analysis was performed on 143 participants with a total of 5246 experience sampling method reports.
Mean predictedEstimateSEt test (df)P valueLoweraUpperb
NoYes
Depressive or anxious symptoms5.194.80.1030.160.64 (136).52−0.2160.421
Gender5.07c4.88d−0.0210.14−0.15 (136).88−0.3020.26
Age group4.89e5f0.6040.23.06 (136).0030.2140.994
Activities
Basic activities4.895.220.1030.061.77 (4254).33−0.6410.848
Relaxation activities4.974.94−0.0720.05−1.31 (4254).42−0.7670.624
Educational activities54.58−0.3680.07−5.23 (4254).12−1.2620.527
Structured activities4.955.370.2660.112.47 (4254).010.0550.476
Instrumental activities4.974.88−0.1440.06−2.39 (4254).02−0.263−0.026
Work activities4.974.82−0.2050.08−2.56 (4254).01−0.362−0.048
MANSAg score (numeric) —h —0.4860.086.34 (136)<.0010.3340.637

aLower: lower bound 95% interval.

bUpper: upper bound 95% interval.

cMale.

dFemale.

eAdolescents (15 and 16 y).

fYoung adults (20 to 24 y).

gMANSA: Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life.

hNot applicable.

Table 4. Average nervousness levels (reverse coded) in adolescents and young adults across 3 Latin American cities. Analysis was performed on 143 participants with a total of 5246 experience sampling method reports.
Mean predictedEstimateSEt test (df)P valueLoweraUpperb
NoYes
Depressive or anxious symptoms2.73.150.1730.20.86 (136).39−0.2270.573
Gender2.82c3.07d0.1440.180.81 (136).42−0.2080.496
Age group5e5.06f−0.4310.25−1.74 (136).08−0.9210.059
Activities
Basic activities3.022.73−0.0920.07−1.4 (4254).39−0.9230.739
Relaxation activities32.910.0090.060.15 (4254).91−0.7630.781
Educational activities2.93.490.5540.087.11 (4254).09−0.4361.545
Structured activities2.972.59−0.1820.12−1.53 (4254).13−0.4150.051
Instrumental activities2.953.010.1170.071.75 (4254).08−0.0140.248
Work activities2.943.290.3870.094.38 (4254)<.0010.2140.561
MANSAg score (numeric)h−0.2730.1−2.85 (136).005−0.463−0.084

aLower: lower bound 95% interval.

bUpper: upper bound 95% interval.

cMale.

dFemale.

eAdolescents (15 and 16 y).

fYoung adults (20 to 24 y).

gMANSA: Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life.

hNot applicable.

Relationship Between Happiness and Nervousness

The full multilevel analysis, which includes various covariates, shows a strong and negative correlation between the predicted values of happiness and nervousness (rs=−0.92; P<.001; n=5246). Additionally, the simple multilevel analysis found that for each point of happiness, the nervousness score decreased by an average of 0.45 points (95% CI −0.48 to −0.42; t3=−41.7; P<.001; SE 0.01).


Main Findings and Comparison With Previous Work

This study used the ESM to examine daily mood status variability (happiness and nervousness) in a sample of adolescents and young adults with (n=86/143, 60%), and without (n= 57/143, 40%) anxiety or depressive symptoms in Buenos Aires, Bogotá, and Lima. Our findings revealed that having depressive or anxiety symptoms is not significantly associated with differences in the variability of daily happiness and nervousness scores.

Although some studies have found that mood variability in patients with mental health problems [Peeters F, Berkhof J, Delespaul P, Rottenberg J, Nicolson NA. Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion. Aug 2006;6(3):383-391. [CrossRef] [Medline]46-Bylsma LM, Taylor-Clift A, Rottenberg J. Emotional reactivity to daily events in major and minor depression. J Abnorm Psychol. Feb 2011;120(1):155-167. [CrossRef] [Medline]48], mainly in adults with major depressive disorder, was higher than in individuals without mental health problems, there is still much debate about the nature of mood fluctuations in this population. Some studies are consistent with our findings, reporting “emotional inertia,” “emotional inflexibility,” or lower mood variability over time in these individuals. For example, Lamers et al [Lamers F, Swendsen J, Cui L, et al. Mood reactivity and affective dynamics in mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127(7):659-669. [CrossRef]49] and Nelson et al [Nelson J, Klumparendt A, Doebler P, Ehring T. Everyday emotional dynamics in major depression. Emotion. 2020;20(2):179-191. [CrossRef]50] found no significant differences in mood variability between those with and without mental health issues. Kuppens et al [Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F. Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. Dec 2010;99(6):1042-1060. [CrossRef] [Medline]27] argue that depressive episodes involve high emotional inertia, implying less mood variability, while Shin et al [Shin KE, Newman MG, Jacobson NC. Emotion network density is a potential clinical marker for anxiety and depression: comparison of ecological momentary assessment and daily diary. Br J Clin Psychol. Jan 2022;61 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):31-50. [CrossRef] [Medline]51] identified that negative experiences correlated with anxiety and depression but did not significantly impact variability.

These mixed findings highlight the complexity of mood variability to mental health, indicating that other factors may also influence these dynamics. Considering this, Pemberton and Tyszkiewicz [Pemberton R, Tyszkiewicz MDF. Factors contributing to depressive mood states in everyday life: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. Aug 2016;200:103-110. [CrossRef] [Medline]52] suggested that the apparent discrepancy between stability and variability in assessments of mood in individuals with mental health problems may be due to the use of different time frames in other studies. It is, therefore, plausible that individuals manifest both short-term mood stability and variability when examined over a more extended period. The mood swings observed in our study may reflect the “short-term stability” of mood in individuals with mental health problems, and it is possible that over a longer period, fluctuations indicating severity or recovery from emotional distress may become more evident. For example, a study of 482 Dutch adolescents followed up 3 times a year for 5 years to observe how their mood varied and how this related to depressive symptoms, delinquent acts, and alcohol consumption. Most of the adolescents showed emotional stability; however, a small group experienced an increase in mood variability during middle adolescence, showing more persistent depressive symptoms and delinquent behaviors [Maciejewski DF, Keijsers L, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. Most fare well-but some do not: distinct profiles of mood variability development and their association with adjustment during adolescence. Dev Psychol. Feb 2019;55(2):434-448. [CrossRef] [Medline]53]. Similarly, 1 study assessed the daily fluctuation in mood (ie, happiness, anger, sadness, and anxiety) with 54 adolescents (9‐12 y) during 4 months at a daily reporting interval. Results evidenced “relatively stable” levels of mood during the first months of the COVID-19 pandemic [Asscheman JS, Zanolie K, Bexkens A, Bos MGN. Mood variability among early adolescents in times of social constraints: a daily diary study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychol. 2021;12:722494. [CrossRef] [Medline]54]. Another study of 58 early adolescents from the Netherlands was conducted over 7 days to explore associations between social context and adolescents’ daily mood state. Participants were enrolled in 5 to 8 daily prompts, using the Ecological Momentary Assessment. Early adolescents showed mood variability when they are alone than in peripheral company [Walsh RJ, van Buuren M, Hollarek M, Sijtsma H, Lee NC, Krabbendam L. Social contexts, momentary mood and affective variability in early adolescence: an exploratory ecological momentary assessment study. J Early Adolesc. Jan 2024;44(1):59-95. [CrossRef]55].

However, our results regarding emotional variability reveal a notable divergence from existing literature on gender differences in adolescents and young adults [Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]25,Flook L. Gender differences in adolescents’ daily interpersonal events and well-being. Child Dev. 2011;82(2):454-461. [CrossRef] [Medline]56]. Specifically, we found that males exhibited greater mood variability in happiness and nervousness than females. This variability was particularly pronounced in happiness scores compared to females. This observation contrasts with the common finding in the literature that females typically report greater emotional variability. However, it aligns with studies suggesting that males might experience more intense emotional responses to stimuli that induce happiness, excitement, or perceived challenges [Gordillo-León F, Mestas-Hernández L, Pérez-Nieto MA, et al. Diferencias de género en la valoración de la intensidad emocional de las expresiones faciales de alegría y tristeza. Escritos Psicología. Jun 2021;14(1):1-10. [CrossRef]57,Acosta FP, Clavero FH. Predictores de la felicidad y la inteligencia emocional en la educación secundaria. Rev colomb psicol. Jan 1, 2018;27(1):71-84. [CrossRef]58].

Finally, concerning emotional variability, young adult participants exhibited greater variability than adolescents in nervousness scores. Previous studies have shown that greater emotional instability is common during late adolescence and young adulthood, resulting in wider and more rapid mood swings. However, these are not always largely related to psychological or social maladjustment within each adolescent [Larson RW, Moneta G, Richards MH, Wilson S. Continuity, stability, and change in daily emotional experience across adolescence. Child Dev. 2002;73(4):1151-1165. [CrossRef] [Medline]59,Larson R, Csikszentmihalyi M, Graef R. Mood Variability and the Psycho-Social Adjustment of Adolescents Applications of Flow in Human Development and Education. Springer Netherlands; 2014:285-304. [CrossRef] ISBN: 978-94-017-9093-260]. These fluctuations in mood are usually the result of internal or external processes to which a person is more or less sensitive, but they will also involve individual and contextual aspects such as the affective base (the initial emotional state) and the strength of the attractor (the tendency to return to a specific emotional state) [Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F. Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. Dec 2010;99(6):1042-1060. [CrossRef] [Medline]27].

It was identified that there is a significant and negative relationship between the average scores of happiness and nervousness obtained during the follow-up week, meaning that as happiness increases, nervousness tends to decrease, and vice versa [Acosta FP, Clavero FH. Ansiedad y felicidad como estados emocionales contrapuestos en la infancia. EJIHPE. 2018;8(2):65. [CrossRef]61]. This finding aligns with the theoretical understanding that these two emotions are inversely related and suggests that intervening in one emotion could impact the other [Acosta FP, Clavero FH. Ansiedad y felicidad como estados emocionales contrapuestos en la infancia. EJIHPE. 2018;8(2):65. [CrossRef]61]. Recognizing this interplay can inform the development of more effective strategies for emotional well-being, potentially incorporating approaches that address both positive and negative emotions to achieve a more balanced emotional state.

We also found that young adults and those with higher perceived quality of life had higher average happiness scores during the follow-up week. Some studies suggest a reduction in subjective well-being during adolescence, which could be attributed to factors such as social pressure, uncertainty about the future, and other typical changes in this life stage [Orben A, Lucas RE, Fuhrmann D, Kievit RA. Trajectories of adolescent life satisfaction. R Soc Open Sci. Aug 2022;9(8):211808. [CrossRef] [Medline]62-Willroth EC, Atherton OE, Robins RW. Life satisfaction trajectories during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood: findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. J Pers Soc Psychol. Jan 2021;120(1):192-205. [CrossRef] [Medline]64]. As individuals transition into adulthood, they often develop new identities and relationship patterns, potentially leading to increased cohesion and commitment to personal and social goals [Marquez J, Long E. A global decline in adolescents’ subjective well-being: a comparative study exploring patterns of change in the life satisfaction of 15-year-old students in 46 countries. Child Indic Res. Jun 2021;24(3):1251-1292. [CrossRef] [Medline]65]. Additionally, previous research indicates that higher subjective well-being is associated with more favorable perceptions of various life aspects, such as health, interpersonal relationships, and the work environment [Maes IHL, Delespaul PAEG, Peters ML, et al. Measuring health-related quality of life by experiences: the experience sampling method. Value Health. Jan 2015;18(1):44-51. [CrossRef] [Medline]66]. This supports our findings, suggesting that lower quality of life scores are related to higher nervousness levels.

For daily activities reported in the ESM questionnaire by participants during the follow-up week, engaging in structured activities (such as playing sports, doing art activities, having group activities with others, etc) was positively related to happiness scores. This is consistent with previous literature using the ESM, where adolescents reported feeling happier during structured leisure activities due to the opportunity to develop activities of interest and the positive impact on mood improvement in everyday life, and found that time spent with peers particularly reduces symptoms of anxiety and sadness [Albertos A, Osorio A, Beltramo C, Albertos A, Osorio A, Beltramo C. Adolescentes y ocio: desarrollo positivo y transición hacia la vida adulta. Educ Educ. Aug 4, 2020;23(2):201-210. [CrossRef]67,Spector WD, Katz S, Murphy JB, Fulton JP. The hierarchical relationship between activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40(6):481-489. [CrossRef] [Medline]68].

Finally, doing instrumental activities (eg, managing finances, cleaning the home, shopping, preparing meals, etc) was negatively related to happiness, and performing work activities was related to lower happiness and higher nervousness levels. These results could be explained by the fact that instrumental activities may be perceived as a burden or an obligation rather than as rewarding or satisfying [Spector WD, Katz S, Murphy JB, Fulton JP. The hierarchical relationship between activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40(6):481-489. [CrossRef] [Medline]68-Fave AD, Bassi M. The quality of experience in adolescents’ daily lives: developmental perspectives. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. Aug 2000;126(3):347-367. [Medline]71]. Similarly, work activities can be a source of stress and worry for many people. This may be due to work pressure, job demands, or lack of job satisfaction.

Strengths and Limitations

Using the ESM, this study collects real-time data on emotional and behavioral states in real contexts from young people in urban areas in middle-income countries. The use of the ESM to explore variability in mood was fruitful, as it allowed us to analyze temporal and contextual patterns that, although associative, suggest that engagement in activities (eg, work and instrumental) is important for reducing distress in young people’s daily lives. Additionally, our sample size was relatively higher compared to other similar studies [Asscheman JS, Zanolie K, Bexkens A, Bos MGN. Mood variability among early adolescents in times of social constraints: a daily diary study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychol. 2021;12:722494. [CrossRef] [Medline]54,Walsh RJ, van Buuren M, Hollarek M, Sijtsma H, Lee NC, Krabbendam L. Social contexts, momentary mood and affective variability in early adolescence: an exploratory ecological momentary assessment study. J Early Adolesc. Jan 2024;44(1):59-95. [CrossRef]55], enabling us to understand the variability in mood status, however, generalizability should be treated with caution.

However, it is important to consider some study limitations when interpreting the results. A limitation of this study is that data collection occurred an average of 107.6 (SD 29.7) days after participants completed the baseline OLA cohort questionnaire. This time lag may have affected the accuracy of characterizing participants, particularly in differentiating between those with and without depressive or anxiety symptoms, as the data may not fully reflect their emotional states before the start of the week of data collection. Although emotional state was assessed using 2 self-report questions with Likert-type scales to capture different mood extremes (eg, sad-happy or nervous-relaxed), this methodology, although friendly due to the frequency of daily notifications, could have led to participants responding in a way that projected a positive self-image or was aligned with perceived expectations. Furthermore, although the activities performed were assessed, the quality of these experiences (positive or negative) was not considered, nor were other factors that might interact with the activities to influence mood, such as competence or self-esteem.

Finally, although compliance rates were satisfactory, problems with receiving notifications may have affected the participation of some individuals due to issues such as poor or no internet connection during the eMoodie app download, specific technical problems with eMoodie on certain smartphone models, or battery-related problems.

Implications in Public Health

The findings from this study underscore the importance of developing customized approaches for enhancing emotional well-being, considering the interplay between demographic factors, quality of life, and emotional variability. Further research is needed to investigate how elements such as gender, age, and the nature of daily activities affect emotional experiences. For clinical practice, these results suggest that interventions may be more successful if they promote engagement in structured activities and focus on improving overall quality of life. Understanding variations in emotional responses across different demographic groups can inform the creation of more targeted and individualized strategies. While it is well known that adolescents experience variability in mood state, professionals need to acknowledge that mood variability should not relate to psychological or social maladjustment, given that mood swings seem to be an inherent aspect of adolescent lifestyle [Larson R, Csikszentmihalyi M, Graef R. Mood Variability and the Psycho-Social Adjustment of Adolescents Applications of Flow in Human Development and Education. Springer Netherlands; 2014:285-304. [CrossRef] ISBN: 978-94-017-9093-260]. From a policy standpoint, ensuring fair access to opportunities and activities that support emotional health is essential. Policies facilitating access to structured and fulfilling activities could enhance emotional well-being and help mitigate differences in emotional experiences among various groups.

Conclusions

Our study reveals that depressive and anxiety symptoms do not significantly affect the variability in predicted happiness and nervousness scores. However, we observed that demographic factors, such as gender and age, play a role in emotional variability, with males and individuals aged 20 to 24 years exhibiting greater nervousness fluctuations and males showing higher variability in happiness. Additionally, young adults reported higher average happiness scores than adolescents, and higher quality of life was associated with greater happiness. Structured activities were linked to increased happiness, whereas instrumental and work-related activities were associated with lower happiness and higher nervousness. These findings highlighted the importance of considering demographic factors and the nature of daily activities when evaluating emotional well-being and suggested that enhancing quality of life and engaging in structured activities could contribute positively to emotional health.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants of this study for sharing their data, experiences, and views with us. This study was funded by the Medical Research Council (MR/S03580X/1).

Data Availability

The dataset analyzed during this study will be available to researchers beginning 9 months and ending 36 months following paper's publication, on reasonable request from Professor Victoria Bird. Proposals should be directed to v.j.bird@qmul.ac.uk to gain access; data requesters will need to sign a data access agreement.

Authors' Contributions

ALV-E, DV-Z, NM-P, SP, and NH handled the conceptualization and did the investigation for this study. AF-V, CF, KA-S, and FLC curated the data. ALV-E and DV-Z worked on the formal analysis. SP, NH, CG-R, LIB, and FD-C acquired the funding. CF administered the project. SP and FD-C supervised this study. NH and DV-Z validated this study. ALV-E, DV-Z, and NM-P wrote the original draft. ALV-E, DV-Z, NM-P, SP, NH, CF, SF, AF-V, KA-S, FLC, AC, NO, LIB, and FD-C reviewed and edited the writing.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

  1. Roach A, Sureshkumar DS, Elliot K, et al. One-year recovery rates for young people with depression and/or anxiety not receiving treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. Jul 21, 2023;13(7):e072093. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  2. Solmi M, Radua J, Olivola M, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry. Jan 2022;27(1):281-295. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Mental health of adolescents. World Health Organization. URL: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health [Accessed 2025-05-14]
  4. Biswas T, Scott JG, Munir K, et al. Global variation in the prevalence of suicidal ideation, anxiety and their correlates among adolescents: a population based study of 82 countries. EClinicalMedicine. Jul 2020;24:100395. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Sikström S, Kelmendi B, Persson N. Assessment of depression and anxiety in young and old with a question-based computational language approach. Npj Ment Health Res. Jul 24, 2023;2(1):11. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Zhao H, Li L, Zhang X, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of depressive disorders among young people aged 10-24 years, 2010-2019. J Psychiatr Res. Feb 2024;170:47-57. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Santomauro DF, Herrera AMM, Shadid J. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet. Nov 6, 2021;398(10312):1700-1712. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. Jun 2012;38(4):661-671. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK. Depression in adolescence. Lancet. Mar 17, 2012;379(9820):1056-1067. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Gómez-Restrepo C, Bohórquez A, Tamayo Martínez N, et al. Trastornos depresivos y de ansiedad y factores asociados en la población de adolescentes colombianos, Encuesta Nacional de Salud Mental 2015. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría. Dec 2016;45:50-57. [CrossRef]
  11. Y 75 años después una generación optimista lista para transformar el perú y el mundo. UNICEF Perú. URL: https:/​/www.​unicef.org/​peru/​informe/​75-anos-despues-generacion-optimista-lista-transformar-peru-mundo [Accessed 2025-05-14]
  12. Ballarino F. ¿La juventud sin preocupaciones? los problemas de salud mental en la adolescencia UNICEF | para cada infancia. UNICEF Argentina. 2022. URL: https:/​/www.​unicef.org/​argentina/​historias/​la-juventud-sin-preocupaciones-los-problemas-de-salud-mental-en-la-adolescencia [Accessed 2025-05-14]
  13. Colizzi M, Lasalvia A, Ruggeri M. Prevention and early intervention in youth mental health: is it time for a multidisciplinary and trans-diagnostic model for care? Int J Ment Health Syst. 2020;14(1):23. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Tzouvara V, Kupdere P, Wilson K, Matthews L, Simpson A, Foye U. Adverse childhood experiences, mental health, and social functioning: a scoping review of the literature. Child Abuse Negl. May 2023;139:106092. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Verhagen SJW, Hasmi L, Drukker M, van Os J, Delespaul P. Use of the experience sampling method in the context of clinical trials. Evid Based Ment Health. Aug 2016;19(3):86-89. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1-32. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Ebner-Priemer UW, Trull TJ. Ecological momentary assessment of mood disorders and mood dysregulation. Psychol Assess. Dec 2009;21(4):463-475. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Oorschot M, Kwapil T, Delespaul P, Myin-Germeys I. Momentary assessment research in psychosis. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(4):498-505. [CrossRef]
  19. Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, et al. Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. Jun 2018;17(2):123-132. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Kim H, Kim S, Kong SS, Jeong YR, Kim H, Kim N. Possible application of ecological momentary assessment to older adults’ daily depressive mood: integrative literature review. JMIR Ment Health. Jun 2, 2020;7(6):e13247. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Kivelä L, van der Does WAJ, Riese H, Antypa N. Don’t miss the moment: a systematic review of ecological momentary assessment in suicide research. Front Digit Health. 2022;4:876595. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Mölsä ME, Lax M, Korhonen J, Gumpel TP, Söderberg P. The experience sampling method in monitoring social interactions among children and adolescents in school: a systematic literature review. Front Psychol. 2022;13:844698. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Li YM, Hachenberger J, Lemola S. The role of the context of physical activity for its association with affective well-being: an experience sampling study in young adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Aug 23, 2022;19(17):10468. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Aldao A. The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. Mar 2013;8(2):155-172. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Maciejewski DF, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1908-1921. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Neumann A, van Lier PAC, Frijns T, Meeus W, Koot HM. Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent psychopathology: a one-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. Jul 2011;39(5):657-669. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F. Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. Dec 2010;99(6):1042-1060. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Holt NJ. Using the experience-sampling method to examine the psychological mechanisms by which participatory art improves wellbeing. Perspect Public Health. Jan 2018;138(1):55-65. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. de Vries LP, Baselmans BML, Bartels M. Smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment of well-being: a systematic review and recommendations for future studies. J Happiness Stud. 2021;22(5):2361-2408. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Holt NJ, Flórez-Varela Á, Gómez-Restrepo C, et al. Which personal and social resources help adolescents to recover from negative affect in daily life? An experience sampling study. J Child Adolesc Ment Health. Jul 23, 2024:1-22. [CrossRef]
  31. Priebe S, Fung C, Brusco LI, et al. Which resources help young people to prevent and overcome mental distress in deprived urban areas in Latin America? A protocol for a prospective cohort study. BMJ Open. Sep 13, 2021;11(9):e052339. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Liao Y, Skelton K, Dunton G, Bruening M. A systematic review of methods and procedures used in ecological momentary assessments of diet and physical activity research in youth: an adapted STROBE Checklist for Reporting EMA Studies (CREMAS). J Med Internet Res. Jun 21, 2016;18(6):e151. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JRZ, et al. Depression from childhood into late adolescence: influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. J Abnorm Psychol. Nov 2015;124(4):803-816. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Benjet C, Albor YC, Bocanegra ES, et al. Incidence and recurrence of depression from adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal follow-up of the Mexican Adolescent Mental Health Survey. J Affect Disord. Feb 15, 2020;263:540-546. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Human Development Index. Human development reports. United Nations. URL: https://hdr.undp.org/data-center/human-development-index [Accessed 2025-05-27]
  36. Santos ME. Measuring Multidimensional Poverty in Latin America: Previous Experience and the Way Forward. Oxford Poverty & Human Development Initiative; 2013. ISBN: 978-1-907194-53-5
  37. Domoff SE, Banga CA, Borgen AL, et al. Use of passive sensing to quantify adolescent mobile device usage: feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary validation of the eMoodie application. Human Behav and Emerg Tech. Jan 2021;3(1):63-74. URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/25781863/3/1 [Accessed 2025-05-14] [CrossRef]
  38. Gómez-Restrepo C, Diez-Canseco F, Brusco LI, et al. Mental distress among youths in low-income urban areas in South America. JAMA Netw Open. Mar 3, 2025;8(3):e250122. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. Apr 2009;114(1-3):163-173. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Monahan PO, Löwe B. Anxiety disorders in primary care: prevalence, impairment, comorbidity, and detection. Ann Intern Med. Mar 6, 2007;146(5):317-325. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Priebe S, Huxley P, Knight S, Evans S. Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA). Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1999;45(1):7-12. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Edemekong PF, Bomgaars DL, Sukumaran S, Schoo C. Activities of daily living. In: StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing; 2024. [Medline]
  43. Badura P, Hamrik Z, Dierckens M, et al. After the bell: adolescents’ organised leisure-time activities and well-being in the context of social and socioeconomic inequalities. J Epidemiol Community Health. Jul 2021;75(7):628-636. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Mesurado B. Actividad estructurada vs. actividad desestructurada, realizadas en solitario vs. en compañía de otros y la experiencia óptima. Anales de Psicología. 2009;25(2):308-315. URL: https://revistas.um.es/analesps/article/view/87711/84501 [Accessed 2025-06-06]
  45. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. Nov 1996;53(11):1033-1039. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Peeters F, Berkhof J, Delespaul P, Rottenberg J, Nicolson NA. Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion. Aug 2006;6(3):383-391. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Thompson RJ, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH. The everyday emotional experience of adults with major depressive disorder: examining emotional instability, inertia, and reactivity. J Abnorm Psychol. Nov 2012;121(4):819-829. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Bylsma LM, Taylor-Clift A, Rottenberg J. Emotional reactivity to daily events in major and minor depression. J Abnorm Psychol. Feb 2011;120(1):155-167. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Lamers F, Swendsen J, Cui L, et al. Mood reactivity and affective dynamics in mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127(7):659-669. [CrossRef]
  50. Nelson J, Klumparendt A, Doebler P, Ehring T. Everyday emotional dynamics in major depression. Emotion. 2020;20(2):179-191. [CrossRef]
  51. Shin KE, Newman MG, Jacobson NC. Emotion network density is a potential clinical marker for anxiety and depression: comparison of ecological momentary assessment and daily diary. Br J Clin Psychol. Jan 2022;61 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):31-50. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Pemberton R, Tyszkiewicz MDF. Factors contributing to depressive mood states in everyday life: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. Aug 2016;200:103-110. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Maciejewski DF, Keijsers L, van Lier PAC, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ, Koot HM. Most fare well-but some do not: distinct profiles of mood variability development and their association with adjustment during adolescence. Dev Psychol. Feb 2019;55(2):434-448. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  54. Asscheman JS, Zanolie K, Bexkens A, Bos MGN. Mood variability among early adolescents in times of social constraints: a daily diary study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychol. 2021;12:722494. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  55. Walsh RJ, van Buuren M, Hollarek M, Sijtsma H, Lee NC, Krabbendam L. Social contexts, momentary mood and affective variability in early adolescence: an exploratory ecological momentary assessment study. J Early Adolesc. Jan 2024;44(1):59-95. [CrossRef]
  56. Flook L. Gender differences in adolescents’ daily interpersonal events and well-being. Child Dev. 2011;82(2):454-461. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  57. Gordillo-León F, Mestas-Hernández L, Pérez-Nieto MA, et al. Diferencias de género en la valoración de la intensidad emocional de las expresiones faciales de alegría y tristeza. Escritos Psicología. Jun 2021;14(1):1-10. [CrossRef]
  58. Acosta FP, Clavero FH. Predictores de la felicidad y la inteligencia emocional en la educación secundaria. Rev colomb psicol. Jan 1, 2018;27(1):71-84. [CrossRef]
  59. Larson RW, Moneta G, Richards MH, Wilson S. Continuity, stability, and change in daily emotional experience across adolescence. Child Dev. 2002;73(4):1151-1165. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. Larson R, Csikszentmihalyi M, Graef R. Mood Variability and the Psycho-Social Adjustment of Adolescents Applications of Flow in Human Development and Education. Springer Netherlands; 2014:285-304. [CrossRef] ISBN: 978-94-017-9093-2
  61. Acosta FP, Clavero FH. Ansiedad y felicidad como estados emocionales contrapuestos en la infancia. EJIHPE. 2018;8(2):65. [CrossRef]
  62. Orben A, Lucas RE, Fuhrmann D, Kievit RA. Trajectories of adolescent life satisfaction. R Soc Open Sci. Aug 2022;9(8):211808. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Smith MD, Wesselbaum D. Well-being inequality among adolescents and young adults. J Adolesc Health. Apr 2024;74(4):703-712. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Willroth EC, Atherton OE, Robins RW. Life satisfaction trajectories during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood: findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. J Pers Soc Psychol. Jan 2021;120(1):192-205. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Marquez J, Long E. A global decline in adolescents’ subjective well-being: a comparative study exploring patterns of change in the life satisfaction of 15-year-old students in 46 countries. Child Indic Res. Jun 2021;24(3):1251-1292. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Maes IHL, Delespaul PAEG, Peters ML, et al. Measuring health-related quality of life by experiences: the experience sampling method. Value Health. Jan 2015;18(1):44-51. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. Albertos A, Osorio A, Beltramo C, Albertos A, Osorio A, Beltramo C. Adolescentes y ocio: desarrollo positivo y transición hacia la vida adulta. Educ Educ. Aug 4, 2020;23(2):201-210. [CrossRef]
  68. Spector WD, Katz S, Murphy JB, Fulton JP. The hierarchical relationship between activities of daily living and instrumental activities of daily living. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40(6):481-489. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  69. Offer S. Family time activities and adolescents’ emotional well‐being. J Marriage Fam. Feb 2013;75(1):26-41. URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/17413737/75/1 [Accessed 2025-05-27] [CrossRef]
  70. McHale SM, Blocklin MK, Walter KN, Davis KD, Almeida DM, Klein LC. The role of daily activities in youths’ stress physiology. J Adolesc Health. Dec 2012;51(6):623-628. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  71. Fave AD, Bassi M. The quality of experience in adolescents’ daily lives: developmental perspectives. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. Aug 2000;126(3):347-367. [Medline]


CREMAS: Adapted Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Checklist for Reporting Ecological Momentary Assessments Studies
ESM: experience sampling method
ESQ: Experience Sampling Questionnaire
GAD-7: General Anxiety Disorder-7
MANSA: Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life
OLA: building resilience and resources to reduce mental distress in young people in Latin America
PHQ-8: Patient Health Questionnaire-8
STROBE: Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology


Edited by Amaryllis Mavragani; submitted 23.08.24; peer-reviewed by Kyara Aguiar; final revised version received 24.02.25; accepted 25.02.25; published 19.06.25.

Copyright

© Ana L Vilela-Estrada, David Villarreal-Zegarra, Nikol Mayo-Puchoc, Nicola Holt, Ángela Flórez-Varela, Catherine Fung, Karen Ariza-Salazar, Fernando Luis Carbonetti, Sumiko Flores, Adriana Carbonel, Natividad Olivar, Carlos Gomez-Restrepo, Luis Ignacio Brusco, Stefan Priebe, Francisco Diez-Canseco. Originally published in JMIR Formative Research (https://formative.jmir.org), 19.6.2025.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Formative Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://formative.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.