Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Mindfulness and meditation have a rich historical tradition, and a growing scientific base of evidence supports their use in creating positive psychological and neuroplastic changes for practitioners. Although meditation can be taught in various ways, the scientific community has yet to systematically study the impact of different types of meditation on neuropsychological outcomes, especially as it pertains to digital implementation. Therefore, it is critical that the instruction of mindfulness be evidence based because meditation is being used in both scientific and clinical settings.
Objective: This study investigated the use of teacher cueing and the integration of neuroscience education into a meditation program. Compassion cueing was chosen as the element of experimental manipulation because traditional lineages of Buddhist meditation teach compassion for self and others as one of the primary outcomes of meditation. We hypothesized that participants receiving compassion cueing would have enhanced neuropsychological outcomes compared with those receiving functional cueing and that gains in neuroscience knowledge would relate to positive neuropsychological outcomes.
Methods: Participants (n=89) were randomized to receive either functional cueing (control group) or compassion cueing (experimental group) and engaged with five 10-minute meditation sessions a week for 4 weeks. All intervention sessions were administered through digital presentation. All participants completed ecological momentary assessments before and after the daily intervention, as well as pre- and postintervention questionnaires.
Results: Participants demonstrated significant benefits over time, including increased mindfulness and self-compassion, decreased depression, and gains in neuroscience content (all P<.001); however, no significant between-group differences were found. Daily scores from each day of the intervention showed a statistically significant shift from active toward settled. Importantly, long-term increases in mindfulness were positively correlated to changes in compassion (r=0.326; P=.009) and self-compassion (r=0.424; P<.001) and negatively correlated to changes in anxiety (r=–0.266; P=.03) and depression (r=–0.271; P=.03). Finally, the acute effects of meditation were significantly correlated to the longitudinal outcomes (with a small-to-medium effect size), especially those relevant to mindfulness.
Conclusions: We developed a novel neuroscience-based education–meditation program that enhanced self-regulation as evidenced by improved mindfulness, self-compassion, and mood state. Our findings demonstrate the behavioral importance of engaging with mindfulness meditation and reinforce the idea that the benefits of meditation are independent of teacher cueing behavior. Future studies will need to investigate the brain-based changes underlying these meditation-induced outcomes.
doi:10.2196/40135
Keywords
Introduction
Background
Mindfulness and meditation have a rich historical evidence base, originating in Asia as part of Buddhist and yogic traditions [
]. The yoga sutras of Patanjali, a collection of Sanskrit statements on the theory and practice of yoga, define mindfulness as the awareness of self [ ]. In contemporary Buddhism, Bodhi [ ] traces the history of mindfulness back to the Pali canon (written in the first century BCE), a canonical text foundational to Buddhist traditions. A modern interpretation of the Pali canon describes mindfulness as “lucid awareness.” In the 1970s, Kabat-Zinn [ ], the founder of the Center for Mindfulness at the University of Massachusetts Memorial Medical Center, defined mindfulness as “...the awareness that emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by moment.” This modern definition of mindfulness separates spirituality from the established baseline set of characteristics used to define the psychological state of mindfulness (eg, paying attention to the present moment) [ ].Meditation is a self-regulatory practice of the body and mind that can be performed in various ways and is a nonpharmaceutical low-cost tool that can be integrated into a healthy lifestyle [
]. A host of literature has examined the effects of meditation on a range of psychological factors and brain outcomes, with multiple systematic reviews and meta-analyses published in this field [ - ]. Meditation substantially promotes the psychological outcome of mindfulness [ ]. Furthermore, meditation decreases symptoms of anxiety and depression [ , ]; improves executive functioning, including attention [ ]; and enhances self-compassion [ ]. Although meditation is often examined in healthy populations, the benefits of meditation are currently being studied in specific clinical populations such as those with psychiatric illness and substance use disorder [ ]. Various studies have used neuroimaging techniques to probe the neural mechanisms of meditation, finding statistically significant increases in activation of the insula, a brain region involved in self-awareness, empathy, and interoception [ ]. The literature also supports decreased amygdala activation paired with increased anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortex activation, indicating that meditation may provide effects at both the bottom-up and top-down levels [ , ]. Researchers theorize that these meditation-induced neural changes may support psychological outcomes, including increased present-moment focus and decreased self-referential thinking or rumination [ ]. In addition, meditation has been shown to alter resting state functional connectivity in the default mode and dorsal attention networks, which is suggested to contribute to the clinical benefits of meditation, including decreases in depression and anxiety [ , ]. Although the size of the scientific literature supporting the benefits of meditation is increasing, it is currently unknown how specific teaching practices within meditation may augment the psychological outcomes.The use of mindful meditation is not standardized [
], and no professional or regulatory body exists to support teaching or implementing mindfulness interventions in a scientific or clinical setting [ ]. Farias and Wikholm [ ] highlight the multitude of mental and physical outcomes of mindfulness [ , ] but note that to optimize outcomes, physical and mental health practitioners will require specific training and certification in mindfulness techniques. It is recommended that instructors of mindful meditation have embodied experience with the practice [ ]; however, translating personal experiences into clinical interventions of mindfulness requires careful consideration. Lessons learned from a clinical experience in the United Kingdom outline six domains of teacher competence: (1) coverage and pacing of session curriculum, (2) relational skills, (3) guiding mindfulness practices, (4) conveying course themes through teaching, (5) embodiment of mindfulness, and (6) management of group process [ ]. In their handbook, Resources for Teaching Mindfulness, McCown et al [ ] suggest that scientific literacy is also a foundational competency for teachers of mindful meditation [ ]. However, it is unknown whether scientific literacy about mindful meditation changes participant outcomes.Objectives
In this single-blind randomized controlled trial (RCT), we explored how teachers of mindfulness use their words and curriculum to guide and instruct meditation to optimize psychological outcomes, including mindfulness, compassion, self-compassion, and mental health. Although meditation can be taught in many different ways, we chose compassion cueing as the element to manipulate because traditional lineages of Buddhist meditation teach compassion for self and others as one of the primary outcomes of meditation [
, ]. In addition, this intentionally scaffolded 4-week meditation program was designed within the framework of a neuroscience education curriculum. All participants learned about the neuroscience of meditation, with a special focus on attentional control, emotional regulation, self-awareness, and self-regulation, before engaging in the actual practice of meditation. We hypothesized that there would be a statistically significant correlation between the gains in neuroscience knowledge and our psychological outcomes of interest. Before and after meditation, daily check-ins provided information about the acute effects of meditation on thoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations. We hypothesized that larger acute gains would correlate to larger long-term gains from meditation, which is yet another unexplored area of research. Understanding the impact of the curricular implementation of meditation will help to fill the gap in best practices and competencies for inclusion in future mindfulness interventions.Methods
Participants
To be eligible for the study, participants were required to be aged ≥18 years and be fluent in English. The exclusion criteria were active trauma or diagnosed and untreated psychiatric illness, which were both self-reported through an initial screening questionnaire developed by the investigators. Recruitment was conducted using social media platforms, other web-based forums, and through in-person announcements by Virginia Tech faculty members who were teaching related content (ie, mindfulness and meditation). The recruitment language included an invitation to practice meditation in the participant’s own home, learn the neuroscience supporting mindfulness meditation, and contribute to new scientific discoveries. Participants were compensated up to US $25 for their participation, with proration occurring at US $2.50 for the pretest assessment, US $1 for each day of meditation, and US $2.50 for the posttest assessment.
Ethics Approval
The institutional review board at Virginia Tech reviewed and approved this study (IRB-20-799), and all participants completed informed consent.
Study Design
This study was a 4-week RCT, with participants randomized to receive either meditation with functional cues (control group) or meditation with self-compassion cues (experimental group). Participants were randomly assigned to the groups through a web-based random number generator. Participants completed pre- and posttest questionnaires along with daily acute assessments. This RCT was partially blinded because the investigators knew participant group assignment; however, participants were unaware of their group assignment. The study included 10-minute meditation sessions 5 days a week. Each day, participants were instructed to watch a 10-minute prerecorded education-meditation video. The control group received standard functional meditation directions (ie, “If you are distracted, return to the mantra.”), whereas the experimental group received functional meditation directions and additional self-compassion cues (ie, “When you are distracted, remember that’s okay. Try to return to the mantra.”). Both groups received identical neuroscience education portions of this study. Participants engaged in neuropsychological assessments before and after the 4-week intervention. In addition, participants completed momentary assessments before and after the presentation of the daily education-meditation video.
The weekly schedule included a scaffolded curriculum and was the same for each of the 4 weeks. Day 1 included 7 minutes of neuroscience education and 3 minutes of meditation. Days 2, 3, and 4 included 5 minutes of neuroscience education and 5 minutes of meditation. The final day of the week was a 10-minute meditation practice (
). The scaffolding of the meditation experience was specifically designed to develop meditation skills over the course of the week, building toward the culminating 10-minute practice; this design is also an example of the flipped classroom model [ ].The education-meditation curriculum was designed by a PhD neuroscientist and an experienced meditation teacher with >10,000 hours of teaching experience. The first week of the curriculum focused on attentional control and used focused attention meditation. Focused attention meditation used the mantra or repeated phrase, “I am alive. I am at ease.” The second week of the curriculum focused on emotional control and used an open monitoring meditation. This technique, called movie of the mind, instructed participants to watch their thoughts without engaging with, or trying to change, them. The third week of the curriculum focused on self-awareness and used the practice of embodiment or connecting mind and body through intentional movement with awareness. Hand turning paired with the breath facilitated mindful embodiment as participants were guided to feel and respond to bodily sensations. The fourth week of the curriculum focused on self-regulation and used the practice of breath control. Participants were instructed to observe their breath without interrupting it (
).Day | Minute | |||||||||
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |
1 | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation |
2 | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation |
3 | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation |
4 | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Neuroscience education | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation |
5 | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation | Meditation |
Week 1 | Week 2 | Week 3 | Week 4 | |
Neurosciencea | Attentional control | Emotional regulation | Self-awareness | Self-regulation |
Meditation styleb | Focused attention | Open monitoring | Embodiment | Breath control |
Meditation technique | Mantra | Movie of the mind | Hand turning | Observing the breath |
aAll groups received the same neuroscience curriculum.
bDuring meditation, the control group received functional cues, and the experimental group received functional cues plus compassion cues.
Regarding the weekly flow of the experiment, participants received an email (on a Sunday) with a link to begin their education-meditation program on the following day. Clicking on the provided link brought the participants to a Qualtrics survey with 3 daily questions:
- “How are your thoughts?”
- “How are your feelings?”
- “How is your body?”
The questions were answered on a standard 10-point Likert scale ranging from 1=“Settled” to 10=“Active.” This set of questions, although not validated, showed sensitivity to change in response to daily interventions. These questions were followed by a prerecorded video with neuroscience education content and lightly guided meditation, followed by a repeat of the daily questions. Once the second set of daily questions was complete, Qualtrics automatically sent the next day’s link 12 hours later. This system allowed participants flexibility to fit their meditations into the most convenient and reliable time during their day. If participants missed a session, reminder emails were sent to them to complete the sessions over the weekend to enable them to complete 5 sessions every 7 days.
Time spent watching the 10-minute video was monitored in Qualtrics with a clock feature on the embedded video page: <10 minutes meant that the participants did not complete the session, and >10 minutes suggested that they may have been distracted with other tasks or fallen asleep. Participants displaying inconsistent times more than once (3/89, 3%) were removed from the study.
Study Measures
Beck Anxiety Inventory
The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) is a self-report measure of anxiety symptoms [
]. Questions about somatic and psychological experiences related to anxiety are included. The BAI includes 21 items for reflection on a 4-point Likert scale, with 0 indicating “Not at all” and 3 indicating “It bothered me a lot.” The total score is calculated by summing responses for each question. Results range from 0 to 21=low anxiety, 22 to 35=moderate anxiety, and ≥36=potentially concerning anxiety levels. The BAI demonstrates high internal consistency, with Cronbach α=.91 and median item correlations at r=0.56. Principal components analysis with eigenvalues >1.0 with a varimax rotation converged in 19 iterations resulted in 5 factors, which accounted for 60% of the variance.Beck Depression Inventory
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) is a self-report measure of depression symptoms, with >25 years of validity testing [
]. The BDI has 21 items, presented in a multiple-choice response format, with the participant selecting the phrase that best reflects their response to the prompt (eg, “I do not feel sad,” “I feel sad,” “I am sad all the time, and I can’t snap out of it,” or “I am so sad and unhappy that I can’t stand it”). Each possible response has an assigned value from 0 to 3, summed for a total score. The sums are then rated as follows: 1 to 10=these ups and downs are considered normal, 11 to 16=mild mood disturbance, 17 to 20=borderline clinical depression, 21 to 30=moderate depression, 31 to 40=severe depression, and >40=extreme depression. The reliability of the BDI was tested in clinical (Cronbach α=.86) and nonclinical (Cronbach α=.80) populations. The test-retest reliability demonstrated r>0.60. Concurrent validity with the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression showed r=0.72 to 0.73 for clinical populations and r=0.60 to 0.74 for nonclinical populations [ ].Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire
The Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) is a self-report measure of trait mindfulness behaviors and mindful thought patterns (Baer et al [
]). It is valid and reliable, demonstrating Cronbach α values ranging from .72 to .92. Confirmatory factor analysis with principal axis factoring with oblique rotation along with scree plots suggests a 5-factor structure. The FFMQ has 39 items and uses a 5-point Likert scale, with 1 representing “Never or very rarely true” and 5 representing “Very often or always true.” The 5 subscales include observing, describing, acting with awareness, nonreacting, and nonjudging. Scoring uses the total sum and sums of subscales, with specific questions scored in reverse.Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) measures core qualities of mindfulness and consciousness [
]. The MAAS consists of 15 items without subscales. Items are scored by individuals on a 6-point Likert scale, with 1 representing “Almost always” and 6 representing “Almost never.” The MAAS was tested in various populations, with Cronbach α values ranging from .80 to .87; after final modifications, the internal consistency of the 15 items showed a Cronbach α value of .82. In a general adult population, the Cronbach α value was .87. Known group validity showed sensitivity to Zen meditators, clinical populations, and the general adult population.Self-Compassion Scale
The Self-Compassion Scale (SCS) was created by Neff [
] as a self-report measure of kindness and understanding toward oneself during times of struggle. The SCS is a 26-item scale measured on a 5-point Likert scale, with 1 representing “Almost never” and 5 representing “Almost always.” The SCS has 6 subscales: self-kindness, self-judgment (reverse scored), common humanity, isolation (reverse scored), mindfulness, and overidentification (reverse scored). Scoring is completed by calculating the means of each subscale, reversing where indicated, and then taking the sum of the subscale means. Cronbach α (.92) shows high internal consistency, with Cronbach α values ranging from .75 to .81 for each of the 6 subscales. Test-retest reliability is high (r=0.93), with subscale values ranging from r=0.80 to r=0.88. Exploratory structural equation modeling found a good fit with 6 factors, with 95% of the variance being attributed to the general factor.Compassion Scale
The Compassion Scale (CS) is a self-report measure of one’s kindness and desire to lessen the struggle of others [
]. The CS includes 16 items divided among 4 subscales: kindness, common humanity, mindfulness, and indifference (reverse scored). The overall score is a total mean. Subscales are also represented as means. A variety of studies show the CS to be reliable, with Cronbach α values ranging from .77 to .90. Test-retest reliability demonstrated r=0.81. Known group validity showed marked differences, as expected in meditators versus nonmeditators. Structural equation modeling found a good fit with 3 positive subscales and 1 negative subscale.Neuroscience Knowledge Check
To assess neuroscience knowledge, we created the Neuroscience Knowledge Check (NKC) to partner with content presented in the education portion of this study (
). This knowledge check helps to determine whether neuroscience knowledge acquisition influences mental health, compassion, and mindfulness outcomes. The NKC consists of 50 items, with 4 subscales paired with one of each of the 4 meditation techniques. Each question offered 4 options: 2 incorrect answers, 1 correct answer, and a selection for “I don’t know.” The inclusion of “I don’t know” as an answer offered participants an honest way to report what they know rather than having participants guess at the correct answer. The total score is a sum of the number of correct answers.Power and Statistical Analysis
An a priori power analysis was run using G*Power (version 3.1; Heinrich Heine University) to determine the appropriate number of participants to power this study. We used an F test, repeated measures ANOVA, within-between interaction using an effect size of 0.25, an α error probability of .0001 to correct for multiple testing, a power level of 0.8, 2 groups (functional cueing vs compassion cueing), 2 measurements (before the intervention vs after the intervention), correlation among repeated measures of 0.5, and a nonsphericity correction epsilon of 1 to determine a sample size of n=98.
Repeated measures ANOVAs with within-between interactions were conducted to examine the hypothesis that teacher behavior (ie, compassion cueing) significantly enhanced mindfulness (ie, FFMQ and MAAS), compassion (ie, CS and SCS), and mental health (ie, BAI and BDI) outcomes in the experimental group compared with the control group. We additionally used Pearson product-moment correlations to test the hypothesis that greater gains in neuroscience knowledge (ie, NKC) would be significantly associated with greater changes in mindfulness, compassion, and mental health outcomes and further that these outcomes of interest would be significantly correlated to one another. Paired samples 2-tailed t tests were used to assess the acute effects of meditation from before to after each session. Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to determine differences among the types of acute changes (ie, thoughts, emotions, and body) across the 20 days of the intervention. An α value of P<.05 was used to determine statistical significance. To correct for multiple testing in a family of analyses, Bonferroni corrections were used as appropriate. SPSS software (version 27.0; IBM Corp) was used for all statistical analyses. Because of technical issues in randomization procedures (ie, Qualtrics was programmed to present 6 rather than 7 questionnaires), participants received 6 of 7 postintervention neuropsychological assessments, resulting in different numbers of participant responses reported in the results.
Results
Participants
Of the 89 participants in this study, 65 (73%) were women, 67 (75%) identified as White, and 52 (58%) were novice meditators. The participants represent diverse annual income, education, and marital status (
). Of the original 127 participants recruited for the study, 35 (39%) dropped out of the study before completion of the posttest assessment, and 3 (3%) were removed for lack of fidelity to research protocols, leaving a total of 89 participants.Category | Participants, n (%) | ||
Sex | |||
Female | 65 (73) | ||
Male | 23 (26) | ||
Intersex | 1 (1) | ||
Race | |||
Asian | 13 (15) | ||
Black or African American | 5 (6) | ||
Other | 4 (4) | ||
White | 67 (75) | ||
Ethnicity | |||
Hispanic | 2 (2) | ||
Non-Hispanic | 87 (98) | ||
Education | |||
Advanced degree | 65 (73) | ||
Bachelor’s degree | 19 (21) | ||
Some college | 4 (4) | ||
Finished high school | 1 (1) | ||
Meditation experience | |||
None | 14 (16) | ||
Novice | 52 (58) | ||
Intermediate | 20 (22) | ||
Advanced | 2 (2) | ||
Expert | 1 (1) | ||
Marital status | |||
Divorced | 8 (9) | ||
Living with significant other | 10 (11) | ||
Married | 44 (49) | ||
Single (never married) | 26 (29) | ||
Widow or widower | 1 (1) | ||
Annual income (US $) | |||
<15,000 | 3 (3) | ||
15,000 to 24,999 | 11 (12) | ||
25,000 to 34,999 | 7 (8) | ||
35,000 to 49,999 | 14 (16) | ||
50,000 to 74,999 | 6 (7) | ||
75,000 to 99,999 | 14 (16) | ||
100,000 to 149,000 | 14 (16) | ||
150,000 to 199,999 | 8 (9) | ||
≥200,000 | 11 (12) | ||
Employment status | |||
Homemaker | 4 (4) | ||
Not working | 7 (8) | ||
Retired | 13 (15) | ||
Working full time | 46 (52) | ||
Working part time | 19 (21) |
Study Measures Assessed Before and After the Intervention
No significant interaction (time × group) effect was found for any of our primary outcomes of interest (
). However, a significant time effect was found for mindfulness ( A; FFMQ: F1,75=26.595; P<.001; partial η2=0.262), with mindfulness scores significantly increasing from before the intervention to after the intervention. This effect was driven by the FFMQ subscales of observing (F1,75=12.680; P<.001; partial η2=0.145), describing (F1,75=10.566; P=.002; partial η2=0.123), acting with awareness (F1,75=5.706; P=.02; partial η2=0.071), nonjudging (F1,75=8.108; P=.006; partial η2=0.098), and nonreactivity (F1,75=24.622; P<.001; partial η2=0.247).In addition, a significant time effect was found for self-compassion (
B; SCS: F1,70=26.406; P<.001; partial η2=0.274), with self-compassion scores significantly increasing from before the intervention to after the intervention. This effect was driven by the SCS subscales of self-kindness (F1,70=15.942; P<.001; partial η2=0.185), self-judgment (F1,70=5.149; P=.03; partial η2=0.069), isolation (F1,70=14.091; P<.001; partial η2=0.168), mindfulness (F1,70=16.070; P<.001; partial η2=0.187), and overidentification (F1,70=169.324; P<.001; partial η2=0.708).A significant time effect was also found for depression (
C; BDI: F1,78=32.852; P<.001; partial η2=0.296), with depression scores significantly decreasing from before the intervention to after the intervention.Finally, a significant time effect was found for neuroscience knowledge (
D; NKC: F1,87=126.387; P<.001; partial η2=0.592), with scores increasing from before the intervention to after the intervention.Neither time effects nor time × group effects were significant for anxiety (BAI), compassion (CS), or dispositional mindfulness (MAAS;
).Study Measures Assessed Before and After the Daily Education-Meditation Practice
As no significant time × group differences were established for measurements assessed before and after the intervention, the following data were analyzed across groups. Daily scores were significantly lower after the meditation than those before the meditation on all days (P<.001), demonstrating a significant acute effect of the daily education-meditation practice from a state of active toward settled (
; ).Change scores from before to after the daily education-meditation practice demonstrated a statistically significant effect over the 20 days of the intervention (
; F15.044,4005.997=5.140; P<.001; partial η2=0.019); however, no time × type (ie, thoughts, emotions, and body) differences were found (F30.007,4005.997=0.573; P=.97; partial η2=0.004).As the intervention provided a unique meditation each week, we additionally visualized and analyzed the data on a per-week basis (
). When each week was analyzed independently, no between-group effects were found for week 1 (F2,267=2.272; P=.11), week 2 (F2,267=1.520; P=.22), week 3 (F2,267=2.026; P=.13), or week 4 (F2,267=1.910; P=.15). However, when data were analyzed across weeks, both time effects (F2.678,715.034=3.389; P=.02; partial η2=0.013) and time × type effects (F5.356,715.034=5.606; P<.001; partial η2=0.040) were found. Post hoc analyses revealed that although the acute effects on thoughts (F3,356=1.409; P=.24) and emotions (F3,356=1.850; P=.14) did not differ per week, the effect on body differed per week (F3,356=2.780; P=.04), with the strongest effect on body occurring during week 1 (comparison with week 2: –1.911, 95% CI –3.362 to –0.456; P=.01; comparison with week 3: –1.578, 95% CI –3.033 to –0.123; P=.03; and comparison with week 4: –1.678, 95% CI –3.133 to –0.223; P=.02).Relationships Among Meditation Outcomes
Regarding the relationship among meditation outcomes, statistically significant correlations were found between the change in FFMQ and the change in CS (r=0.326; P=.009), SCS (r=0.424; P<.001), BAI (r=–0.266; P=.03), and BDI (r=–0.271; P=.03;
). In addition, statistically significant correlations were found between the change in MAAS and the change in SCS self-judgment (r=0.333; P=.008), BAI (r=–0.528; P<.001), and BDI (r=–0.314; P=.008; ). The change in neuroscience knowledge revealed only 1 significant correlation with SCS common humanity (r=0.287; P=.02). All other correlations regarding meditation outcomes are provided in .Furthermore, total daily change scores (after meditation minus before meditation) demonstrated significant correlations with the change in FFMQ (r=–0.251; P=.03), FFMQ acting with awareness (r=–0.235; P=.04), FFMQ nonreactivity (r=–0.239; P=.04), MAAS (r=–0.352; P=.002), and SCS mindfulness (r=–0.269; P=.03). In addition, total daily change score for the subscale of emotions was significantly correlated to the change in SCS (r=–0.258; P=.03). All other correlations regarding specific aspects of total daily change scores related to thoughts, emotions, and body are provided in
.Discussion
Principal Findings
This study examined the effect of adding compassion cueing to a neuroscience-based mindfulness meditation practice on various neuropsychological outcomes. We found that although compassion cueing did not enhance our outcomes of interest, the practice of meditation enhanced mindfulness, increased self-compassion, and decreased levels of depression. Importantly, we found that those individuals who gained the most in terms of mindfulness showed the largest gains in compassion, self-compassion, and mental health. In addition, we found that although our intervention improved neuroscience knowledge, this new knowledge was not correlated to our neuropsychological outcomes of interest. Finally, we found that the acute effects of meditation were related to the long-term effects of meditation: those who gained the most in terms of acute effects benefited the most in the long term. Although our neuropsychological findings are consistent with existing literature [
, - ], this is the first time that this novel neuroscience education and meditation program has been shown to improve psychological state. In addition, we newly show that the acute effects of this program demonstrate small yet significant correlations to the longitudinal outcomes.The Effects of Meditation on the Mind
Our results are consistent with existing literature demonstrating that meditation is a powerful tool to improve neuropsychological function, with the most significant positive impacts seen in self-regulatory behaviors [
, ]. Specifically, we found that this novel education-meditation intervention promoted increases in mindfulness, decreases in depression, and gains in self-compassion. However, to truly demonstrate an effect, future studies will need to include a control group that does not receive meditation and another group that does not receive the neuroscience education component. The purpose of this study, however, was to examine ways to fine-tune the effects of meditation on brain function. Together, these psychological outcomes are evidence that this 4-week mindfulness program promotes self-regulation [ , ], which includes aspects of enhanced self-awareness (ie, increased mindfulness) and emotional regulation (ie, decreased depression). Importantly, the outcome of self-compassion reflects internal flexibility, allowing an individual to be caring, kind, and nonjudgmental toward oneself, particularly during times of distress [ ]. This finding indicates that this practice may be especially beneficial for populations with deficits in self-compassion, such as those with neuropsychiatric disorders (eg, major depressive disorder) [ ]. Others have hypothesized that self-compassion is an important indicator of adaptive psychological function associated with mindfulness practices [ , , ]. In fact, it is suggested that as an outcome of mindful meditation [ ], self-regulation allows for greater self-compassion through meditation’s mechanisms of action at the level of the brain [ ].Similar to our work, previous studies indicate that increased mindfulness leads to improved well-being [
, ], decreased depression symptomatology [ ], and reduced stress [ , ]. Well-being and stress reduction are part of preventive health measures that require active intentionality to cultivate [ ]. We hypothesize, along with others, that the intentional practice of meditation may be a key way to improve well-being, with this effect being driven by the impact of meditation on self-regulatory brain processes. Research has demonstrated that enhanced self-regulation can be characterized by neuroplastic changes to the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, temporoparietal junction, frontolimbic network, and default mode network structures [ ]. Therefore, future mindful meditation studies should use neuroimaging techniques to examine how these brain areas are affected and how changes in these brain structures relate to meditation-induced changes in self-regulation.The Effect of Teacher Cueing on Meditation Outcomes
Our novel intervention focused on the teacher’s cueing behavior (ie, functional cueing vs compassion cueing) during meditation and how cueing affected outcomes related to mindfulness, compassion, and mental health. Interestingly, we found that cueing behavior did not have a substantial impact on our outcomes of interest. Rather, we found that it is the practice of meditation itself rather than teacher behavior that affects outcomes. Similar to our work, Condon et al [
] presented mindful meditation to one group and compassion meditation to another over an 8-week period. Participants were 5 times (odds ratio 5.33) more likely after the intervention to offer compassion to others, irrespective of the type of meditation they were assigned.This particular outcome of our study is consistent with mindful self-regulation theory [
], suggesting that mindfulness, no matter how it is practiced, has distinct and predictable outcomes that may improve health behaviors. Crane et al [ ] and Baer and Wolf [ ] also emphasize the importance of the behavior itself, focusing on the elements that precede it and the resulting change through the scientific field of applied behavioral analysis. This study highlights the importance of the actual practice of meditation; no matter how meditation is taught or presented, the act of engaging in meditation results in improved psychological health.Knowing About the Brain Is Not Necessary for Functional Improvements of the Brain
This intervention included a neuroscience education curriculum, teaching the neuroscience of 4 unique meditation techniques that focused on attention, emotional regulation, self-awareness, and self-regulation (
). Our research demonstrates that participants increased their knowledge of the neuroscience that supports these meditation techniques; however, neuroscience knowledge acquisition was not related to improvements in our outcomes of interest (ie, mindfulness, self-compassion, and depression). This lack of correlation further supports the idea that practicing mindfulness meditation has a powerful effect on outcomes without the need to fully understand the process.Although mindfulness has been examined from the perspective of neuroscience [
, ], this is the first meditation intervention designed within the context of a neuroscience curriculum. Specifically, the curriculum used in this intervention intentionally scaffolded neuroscience content and meditation skills. The curriculum built skills considering the prerequisites needed to move on to the following week: attentional control (ie, mantra) was presented first, followed by emotional control (ie, movie of the mind), self-awareness (ie, hand turning), and finally self-regulation (ie, watching the breath). Future RCTs will need to evaluate this meditation intervention with versus without the inclusion of the neuroscience curriculum.The Acute Effects of Meditation Correspond to the Longitudinal Outcomes of Meditation
The daily scores provided insight into how meditation acutely affected thoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations. First, we found that the most prominent acute effects occurred during the first week of the intervention, with thoughts, emotions, and body moving from active to settled from before to after the daily intervention. Subsequently, the total change scores decreased over the 20 days of the intervention; however, this change was related to the long-term beneficial effects of the intervention, that is, as the intervention progressed, participants started their daily intervention in a more settled state, with less room for improvement. This is similar to other work showing that the effects of meditation change as novice meditators gain experience [
, - ]. Regarding the impact of each unique meditation, attentional control demonstrated the most significant acute effects, with this effect being driven by changes in bodily sensations. This may be because it was the first week of meditation. Alternatively, attentional control meditation may have had the most significant impact on acute changes at the level of the body. Although this curriculum was intentionally scaffolded, future studies may choose to alter, in terms of the sequence, the presentation of attentional control, emotional control, self-awareness, and self-regulation meditations.Importantly, we found that the acute effects of meditation substantially related to our outcomes of interest. Specifically, those individuals who showed the largest acute benefits (from states of active to settled) showed the largest gains in various aspects of mindfulness, including acting with awareness, nonreactivity, dispositional mindfulness, and self-compassion mindfulness. In addition, those who demonstrated the largest shifts in emotion from active to settled, demonstrated the largest gains in self-compassion. This is the first study that has combined ecological momentary assessment with long-term outcomes to show that the acute effects of meditation are related to the chronic effects.
Limitations and Future Directions
Although the results of this study are well supported by existing literature and add to existing knowledge, limitations exist. First, this study was conducted on a convenience sample that could have been more diverse. Second, because of technical issues, not all posttest data were collected from every participant, creating unequal sample sizes for some measurements. In addition, many of the predictable neuroplastic changes associated with mindful meditation may take longer to manifest than the duration of 20 days spanning over 4 weeks offered by this intervention. A future iteration of this study should be completed with each of the 4 techniques spanning 2 weeks for a total intervention duration of 8 weeks.
Meditation studies often struggle to have a solid study design and implementation methodology. Among these challenges is finding a suitable control for mindful meditation. We foresee that separating the neuroscience education from the meditation practice will create an excellent mindful meditation education control. In addition, as the intervention did not display any statistically significant differences between the control and experimental groups, future studies should include a comparison of basic cueing versus no cueing.
Finally, this study design included 5 days of meditation spread over a 7-day week. This allowed for the most flexibility from participants. If a day was missed during a traditional weekday, participants could complete their session or sessions on the weekend. Therefore, the effect of the meditation may have been lessened by this break in meditation. In addition, creating a daily routine with less flexibility may prove easier for participants. Future study designs could include (1) 20 consecutive days or (2) a longer intervention with 40 mindful meditation sessions over 8 weeks to investigate the effect of meditation dosage.
Conclusions
This study, which used a novel neuroscience-based education–meditation program, demonstrated the behavioral importance of engaging with mindfulness meditation to optimize individual well-being through improved mindfulness, self-compassion, and depression symptomatology, suggesting enhanced self-regulation. This study additionally reinforces the idea that the benefits of meditation are independent of teacher cueing behavior. These neuropsychological changes are likely supported by neuroplastic changes associated with mindfulness meditation, but future work will need to investigate the brain-based outcomes. Finally, this study newly demonstrates that the acute effects of meditation translate into longitudinal outcomes. Future studies will need to systematically investigate the inclusive use of the neuroscience curriculum as well as test the effects of other types of teacher cueing.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the integrated Translational Health Research Institute of Virginia (iTHRIV) Scholars Program, which is supported in part by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (UL1TR003015 and KL2TR003016). The Statistical Applications and Innovations Group, Department of Statistics, Virginia Tech, also supported this work. Finally, this work was supported by Virginia Tech’s Open Access Subvention Fund.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
Neuroscience knowledge questionnaire (administered both before and after the intervention).
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 72 KB
Mean (SE of the mean) for all neuropsychological outcomes of interest for both groups both before and after the intervention. Time and time × group effects are presented. All statistically significant effects are highlighted in bold.
XLSX File (Microsoft Excel File), 13 KB
Ecological momentary assessment scores from before to after the daily education-meditation practice demonstrated a statistically significant change each day of the intervention. Mean change scores (pre-post assessments) are presented for each day of the intervention. Statistically significant effects are highlighted in bold.
XLSX File (Microsoft Excel File), 12 KB
Correlation table for all outcomes of interest, including both ecological momentary assessments and primary endpoints. All statistically significant effects are highlighted in bold.
XLSX File (Microsoft Excel File), 23 KB
CONSORT-eHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.2).
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 93 KBReferences
- Deshmukh VD. Neuroscience of meditation. ScientificWorldJournal 2006 Nov 16;6:2239-2253 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bharati SV, Zambito S. Unadorned Thread of Yoga: The Yoga-Sutra of Patañjali in English. UK: Yoga-Sutras Institute Press; 2021.
- Bodhi B. What does mindfulness really mean? A canonical perspective. In: Mindfulness. Milton Park, Abingdon-on-Thames, Oxfordshire, England, UK: Routledge; 2013.
- Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2003;10(2):144-156. [CrossRef]
- Brandmeyer T, Delorme A, Wahbeh H. The neuroscience of meditation: classification, phenomenology, correlates, and mechanisms. Prog Brain Res 2019;244:1-29. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Goyal M, Singh S, Sibinga EM, Gould NF, Rowland-Seymour A, Sharma R, et al. Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med 2014 Mar;174(3):357-368 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schlechta Portella CF, Ghelman R, Abdala V, Schveitzer MC, Afonso RF. Meditation: evidence map of systematic reviews. Front Public Health 2021;9:742715 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fox KC, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, Floman JL, Ellamil M, Rumak SP, et al. Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014 Jun;43:48-73. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rahrig H, Vago DR, Passarelli MA, Auten A, Lynn NA, Brown KW. Meta-analytic evidence that mindfulness training alters resting state default mode network connectivity. Sci Rep 2022 Jul 18;12(1):12260 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tang Y, Hölzel BK, Posner MI. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015 Apr 18;16(4):213-225. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chen KW, Berger CC, Manheimer E, Forde D, Magidson J, Dachman L, et al. Meditative therapies for reducing anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Depress Anxiety 2012 Jul;29(7):545-562 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schreiner I, Malcolm J. The benefits of mindfulness meditation: changes in emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress. Behav change 2012 Feb 22;25(3):156-168. [CrossRef]
- Rubia K. The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychol 2009 Sep;82(1):1-11. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Germer CK, Neff KD. Self-compassion in clinical practice. J Clin Psychol 2013 Aug;69(8):856-867. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Dakwar E, Levin FR. The emerging role of meditation in addressing psychiatric illness, with a focus on substance use disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2009;17(4):254-267 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Young KS, van der Velden AM, Craske MG, Pallesen KJ, Fjorback L, Roepstorff A, et al. The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain activity: a systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018 Jan;84:424-433. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wheeler MS, Arnkoff DB, Glass CR. The neuroscience of mindfulness: how mindfulness alters the brain and facilitates emotion regulation. Mindfulness 2017 Jun 21;8(6):1471-1487 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Zhang Z, Luh W, Duan W, Zhou GD, Weinschenk G, Anderson AK, et al. Longitudinal effects of meditation on brain resting-state functional connectivity. Sci Rep 2021 May 31;11(1):11361 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Yang C, Barrós-Loscertales A, Pinazo D, Ventura-Campos N, Borchardt V, Bustamante J, et al. State and training effects of mindfulness meditation on brain networks reflect neuronal mechanisms of its antidepressant effect. Neural Plast 2016;2016:9504642 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Grabovac A, Burrell E. Standardizing training in mindfulness-based interventions in Canadian psychiatry postgraduate programs: a competency-based framework. Acad Psychiatry 2018 Apr;42(2):248-254 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Farias M, Wikholm C. Has the science of mindfulness lost its mind? BJPsych Bull 2016 Dec 02;40(6):329-332 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Baer RA. Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2003;10(2):125-143. [CrossRef]
- Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res 2004 Jul;57(1):35-43. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Woods SL. Training professionals in mindfulness: the heart of teaching. In: Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness. New York: Springer; 2009.
- Crane RS, Kuyken W, Hastings RP, Rothwell N, Williams JM. Training teachers to deliver mindfulness-based interventions: learning from the UK experience. Mindfulness (N Y) 2010 Jun;1(2):74-86 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- McCown D, Reibel D, Micozzi M. Resources for Teaching Mindfulness an International Handbook. Midtown Manhattan, New York City: Springer International Publishing; 2017.
- Goetz JL, Keltner D, Simon-Thomas E. Compassion: an evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychol Bull 2010 May;136(3):351-374 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tirch DD. Mindfulness as a context for the cultivation of compassion. Int J Cogn Ther 2010 Jun;3(2):113-123. [CrossRef]
- Hew KF, Lo CK. Flipped classroom improves student learning in health professions education: a meta-analysis. BMC Med Educ 2018 Mar 15;18(1):38 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Borden JW, Peterson DR, Jackson EA. The beck anxiety inventory in nonclinical samples: initial psychometric properties. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 1991 Dec;13(4):345-356 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 1988 Jan;8(1):77-100. [CrossRef]
- Miller IW, Bishop S, Norman WH, Maddever H. The modified hamilton rating scale for depression: reliability and validity. Psychiatry Res 1985 Feb;14(2):131-142. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 2006 Mar;13(1):27-45. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003 Apr;84(4):822-848. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- NEFF K. Self-compassion: an alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self Identity 2003 Apr;2(2):85-101. [CrossRef]
- Pommier E, Neff KD, Tóth-Király I. The development and validation of the compassion scale. Assessment 2020 Jan;27(1):21-39. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bohlmeijer E, Prenger R, Taal E, Cuijpers P. The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on mental health of adults with a chronic medical disease: a meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res 2010 Jun;68(6):539-544. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. J Altern Complement Med 2009 May;15(5):593-600. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Khoury B, Lecomte T, Fortin G, Masse M, Therien P, Bouchard V, et al. Mindfulness-based therapy: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2013 Aug;33(6):763-771. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Basso JC, McHale A, Ende V, Oberlin DJ, Suzuki WA. Brief, daily meditation enhances attention, memory, mood, and emotional regulation in non-experienced meditators. Behav Brain Res 2019 Jan 01;356:208-220. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schultz P, Ryan R. The “why,” “what,” and “how” of healthy self-regulation: mindfulness and well-being from a self-determination theory perspective. In: Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation. New York, NY: Springer; 2015.
- Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology: Volume 1. Thousand Oaks, California, United States: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2012.
- Athanasakou D, Karakasidou E, Pezirkianidis C, Lakioti A, Stalikas A. Self-compassion in clinical samples: a systematic literature review. Psychology 2020;11(2):217-244. [CrossRef]
- McKay T, Walker BR. Mindfulness, self-compassion and wellbeing. Personal Indiv Diff 2021 Jan;168:110412. [CrossRef]
- Short MM, Mazmanian D, Oinonen K, Mushquash CJ. Executive function and self-regulation mediate dispositional mindfulness and well-being. Personal Indiv Diff 2016 Apr;93:97-103 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Birtwell K, Williams K, van Marwijk H, Armitage CJ, Sheffield D. An exploration of formal and informal mindfulness practice and associations with wellbeing. Mindfulness (N Y) 2019;10(1):89-99 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fish J, Brimson J, Lynch S. Mindfulness interventions delivered by technology without facilitator involvement: what research exists and what are the clinical outcomes? Mindfulness (N Y) 2016;7(5):1011-1023 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Nyklícek I, Kuijpers K. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention on psychological well-being and quality of life: is increased mindfulness indeed the mechanism? Ann Behav Med 2008 Jun;35(3):331-340 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci 2011 Nov;6(6):537-559. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Condon P, Desbordes G, Miller WB, DeSteno D. Meditation increases compassionate responses to suffering. Psychol Sci 2013 Oct;24(10):2125-2127. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schuman-Olivier Z, Trombka M, Lovas DA, Brewer JA, Vago DR, Gawande R, et al. Mindfulness and behavior change. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2020;28(6):371-394 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Baer DM, Wolf MM. Some still-current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. J Appl Behav Anal 1987;20(4):313-327 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tang Y, Leve LD. A translational neuroscience perspective on mindfulness meditation as a prevention strategy. Transl Behav Med 2016 Mar;6(1):63-72 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fox KC, Zakarauskas P, Dixon M, Ellamil M, Thompson E, Christoff K. Meditation experience predicts introspective accuracy. PLoS One 2012;7(9):e45370 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hölzel BK, Ott U, Hempel H, Hackl A, Wolf K, Stark R, et al. Differential engagement of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal cortex in adept meditators and non-meditators. Neurosci Lett 2007 Jun 21;421(1):16-21. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rodriguez-Larios J, Bracho Montes de Oca EA, Alaerts K. The EEG spectral properties of meditation and mind wandering differ between experienced meditators and novices. Neuroimage 2021 Dec 15;245:118669 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
Abbreviations
BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory |
BDI: Beck Depression Inventory |
CS: Compassion Scale |
FFMQ: Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire |
MAAS: Mindful Attention Awareness Scale |
NKC: Neuroscience Knowledge Check |
RCT: randomized controlled trial |
SCS: Self-Compassion Scale |
Edited by A Mavragani; submitted 09.06.22; peer-reviewed by K Uludag, M Ferrari; comments to author 17.07.22; revised version received 09.11.22; accepted 17.11.22; published 19.01.23
Copyright©Sarah Lynn, Julia C Basso. Originally published in JMIR Formative Research (https://formative.jmir.org), 19.01.2023.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Formative Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://formative.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.