Published on in Vol 6, No 12 (2022): December

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/40058, first published .
Analysis of Patient Cues in Asynchronous Health Interactions: Pilot Study Combining Empathy Appraisal and Systemic Functional Linguistics

Analysis of Patient Cues in Asynchronous Health Interactions: Pilot Study Combining Empathy Appraisal and Systemic Functional Linguistics

Analysis of Patient Cues in Asynchronous Health Interactions: Pilot Study Combining Empathy Appraisal and Systemic Functional Linguistics

Original Paper

1Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark

2Liva Healthcare, Copenhagen, Denmark

3Department of Nordic Studies and Linguistics, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

4See Acknowledgements, Dublin, Ireland

Corresponding Author:

Elena Rey Velasco, BSc, MSc

Department of Psychology

University of Copenhagen

Øster Farimagsgade 2A

Copenhagen, 1353

Denmark

Phone: 45 35324800

Email: erv@psy.ku.dk


Background: Lifestyle-related diseases are among the leading causes of death and disability. Their rapid increase worldwide has called for low-cost, scalable solutions to promote health behavior changes. Digital health coaching has proved to be effective in delivering affordable, scalable programs to support lifestyle change. This approach increasingly relies on asynchronous text-based interventions to motivate and support behavior change. Although we know that empathy is a core element for a successful coach-user relationship and positive patient outcomes, we lack research on how this is realized in text-based interactions. Systemic functional linguistics (SFL) is a linguistic theory that may support the identification of empathy opportunities (EOs) in text-based interactions, as well as the reasoning behind patients' linguistic choices in their formulation.

Objective: This study aims to determine whether empathy and SFL approaches correspond and complement each other satisfactorily to study text-based communication in a health coaching context. We sought to explore whether combining empathic assessment with SFL categories can provide a means to understand client-coach interactions in asynchronous text-based coaching interactions.

Methods: We retrieved 148 text messages sent by 29 women who participated in a randomized trial of telecoaching for the prevention of gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM) and postnatal weight loss. We conducted a pilot study to identify users' explicit and implicit EOs and further investigated these statements using the SFL approach, focusing on the analysis of transitivity and thematic analysis.

Results: We identified 164 EOs present in 42.37% (3478/8209) of the word count in the corpus. These were mainly negative (n=90, 54.88%) and implicit (n=55, 60.00%). We distinguished opening, content and closing messages structures. Most of the wording was found in the content (n=7077, 86.21%) with a declarative structure (n=7084, 86.30%). Processes represented 22.4% (n=1839) of the corpus, with half being material (n=876, 10.67%) and mostly related to food and diet (n=196, 54.92%), physical activity (n=96, 26.89%), and lifestyle goals (n=40, 11.20%).

Conclusions: Our findings show that empathy and SFL approaches are compatible. The results from our transitivity analysis reveal novel insights into the meanings of the users’ EOs, such as their seek for help or praise, often missed by health care professionals (HCPs), and on the coach-user relationship. The absence of explicit EOs and direct questions could be attributed to low trust on or information about the coach’s abilities. In the future, we will conduct further research to explore additional linguistic features and code coach messages.

Trial Registration: Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ANZCTR) ACTRN12620001240932; https://www.anzctr.org.au/Trial/Registration/TrialReview.aspx?id=380020

JMIR Form Res 2022;6(12):e40058

doi:10.2196/40058

Keywords



Noncommunicable diseases (NCDs) account for 73.6% of deaths worldwide. These lifestyle-related diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases (CVDs), some cancers, respiratory diseases, and diabetes, are among the most common causes of death and disability [1]. The rapid increase in NCD rates is a global disease burden in both developed and developing nations. However, we now know that these NCDs can be prevented, or substantially delayed, by changes in lifestyle (eg, factors such as diet, physical activity, stress, and sleep), as shown in numerous trials [2-4]. Research has demonstrated that with appropriate individual and group support, individuals can achieve significant weight loss and sustainable changes in lifestyle [5,6]. Nonetheless, the high incidence of NCDs and the limited resources to deliver best-practice behavior change programs make large-scale prevention programs challenging. The need for cost-effective alternatives has led some countries, such as the United States [7] and the United Kingdom [8], to seek new strategies to promote health behavior changes.

To address the challenges of scalability and cost-effectiveness, NCD prevention programs are increasingly using digital technologies. One technology that facilitates access to prevention programs is telehealth, which is the use of video or audio technologies to deliver a health intervention. Telehealth has the potential to reduce health care costs and increase the scope of these programs, as it can substitute or supplement in-person visits when personal attendance is not possible (eg, patients living in rural areas [9,10]). Using telehealth to deliver face-to-face behavior change programs has been shown to be as effective as in-person programs for NCDs [11,12]. For these reasons, the field of telehealth has experienced substantial growth over recent years, and the COVID-19 pandemic has accelerated the process, with programs for mental health, rehabilitation, and medical consultations showing rapid increases in usage [13-15].

In the context of disease prevention programs, this approach is increasingly referred to as telehealth coaching in order to distinguish it from the delivery of more traditional telehealth services. Telehealth coaching uses an integrative health coaching (IHC) approach. IHC connects the coaching intervention with the individual’s personal values and sense of purpose [16]. Instead of being instructed on how to reach their goals, the coach provides the user, or person being coached, with the knowledge, skills, and confidence to perform autonomously [17]. These telehealth coaching programs combine multimodalities of digital technology to support people in achieving their lifestyle goals in a synchronous or asynchronous form. Traditional synchronous interactions use real-time, face-to-face meetings, telephone calls, or video calls [18], and asynchronous interactions consist of the exchange of texts, audio, or video messages that the user can access and review later [19]. At the same time, health coaching allows for only human [14,20], only automated [21,22], or hybrid [23-25] modalities. Although all of these have shown positive results, it is still not clear which one is more effective [26]. A recent meta-analysis showed that automated digital interventions (ADIs) are a good addition to weight loss coaching interventions and results are more effective when the coaching program duration is shorter [27].

Generally, digital health coaching interventions follow a prespecified framework, such as manuals or guidelines, based on the current evidence on behavior change [28] and psychosocial theories [29,30]. Evaluating whether coaches are delivering a program as intended is key to ensuring a homogenous and effective intervention, and telehealth coaching poses unique challenges in this regard due to its multiple modalities [31]. Although there is increasing research exploring the fidelity of such programs, current research work has focused on synchronous, face-to-face interventions delivered by coaches [32-34]. This research typically quantifies the behavior change techniques (BCTs) delivered by the coach and to some extent the way in which these interventions are realized. State-of-the-art findings show a predominant focus on the coaches’ performance and users’ outcomes without accounting for the users’ cues and responses [35], in addition to inconsistencies in fidelity reporting [36]. With increasing drivers for efficiency and the use of responsive artificial intelligence (AI) systems, the use of asynchronous interactions to support health coaching is growing. However, there is little research on these asynchronous interactions and a clear need to understand their nature and how to optimize them. Although asynchronous interventions are delivered through audio or video messages on a digital platform, the most common form of interaction is through the exchange of text messages. A coaching platform can be automatized to send scheduled messages (eg, reminders). There is a body of research on the use of automated messages to remind, prompt, or nudge healthier behavior, which demonstrates their potential [37], effectiveness [38], and language used [39,40] in text-based behavior change interventions. However, these messages represent a 1-way communication from the coaching platform to the individual. The users participating in these telehealth coaching programs also communicate directly to their coach or AI coaching platform. Their text messages can be responded to by an AI-based system (eg, chatbots) or by their coach (ie, individually crafted correspondence).

Nonetheless, there is a wealth of literature on the effectiveness of traditional synchronous, face-to-face patient-provider interactions where researchers share an overall concern for the quality of asynchronous consultations [41], as well as for the quality of the relationship developed [42]. In this context, the concepts of empathy, sympathy, and compassion in health care are crucial in the patient-provider relationship but sometimes confused with one another [43]. Compassion is a deep awareness of another person’s suffering, along with the wish to relieve it [44], whereas empathy is the cognitive skill to understand and “feel with” the patient. Some authors have identified the dimensions of cognitive, affective (relegated to sympathy), and emotional within the definition of empathy [45]. Other authors, such as Piasecki, see clinical empathy as “the ability to understand and participate in another person’s feelings and emotional state, while sympathy describes the listener’s feelings without understanding or sharing the patient’s emotions” [46]. The positive effect of clinical empathy on patient outcomes has been documented across psychological, sociological, therapeutical, and behavioral disciplines [47,48] and should be preserved in text-based, asynchronous interventions. An empathetic response is important for building a therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy, and effective relational skills are essential in behavior change programs for promoting health outcomes. Thereby, an empathic frame is a good start point when coding asynchronous messages. There are a number of tools in patient-provider communication for identifying opportunities for empathic responses [49], showing how providers often miss these opportunities [50], and providing advice to prevent it [51]. According to a review by Epstein et al [52], patient-centered communication (PCC) comprises “(1) eliciting and understanding the patient's perspective—concerns, ideas, expectations, needs, feelings and functioning, (2) Understanding the patient within his or her unique psychosocial context, (3) Reaching a shared understanding of the problem and its treatment with the patient that is concordant with the patient's values, and (4) Helping patients to share power and responsibility by involving them in choices to the degree that they wish.” Epstein’s arguments are present when expressing empathy in a health care context. However, this approach has not been informed by our understandings of language and, in particular, the functions of language.

Pounds [53] presented an empathy appraisal approach, supported by previous discourse analysis studies based on systemic functional linguistics (SFL), to explore the expressions of empathy in PCC. Nonetheless, it is surprising that her approach does not incorporate SFL into this patient-provider communication analysis. According to SFL theory, developed by Halliday and Matthiessen [54], language in itself has a communicative and a meaning potential that is realized through language production, and that language in itself is social semiotics, an approach to communication that aims to comprehend how individuals in particular social contexts interact through a variety of means. The goal of studying communication from this angle is to classify the semiotic decisions that communicators are able to make [55]. The empathy opportunities (EOs) that Pounds considered in her empathy appraisal may be further informed by these choices. Suchman [56] defined implicit EOs as “patient statements from which a clinician might infer an underlying emotion that has not been explicitly expressed” and explicit EOs as “statements about situations or concerns that might plausibly be associated with an emotion.” To fully grasp the meaning potential of the patients’ linguistic choices whenever they express an EO, however, we must first understand the 3 SFL metafunctions that construe that meaning: the ideational metafunction, which describes the speaker’s inner and outer experience; the interpersonal metafunction, which concerns the relationship between the speaker and the recipient as well as between the speaker and their message; and the textual metafunction, which is used to interpret the text as a text and not just as a cluster of words or clauses [57]. The transitivity system is a component of the ideational metafunction and goes further than the distinction between transitive and intransitive verbs. A transitivity analysis explores how the speaker construes their experience of the world. The processes, participants implicated, and circumstances of this experience are all part of the transitivity system. Processes are realized by the verbal group of the clause and can be classified as material, mental, relational, verbal, existential, or behavioral [58]. We provide a further description of the process categories in the Methods section. Several researchers have chosen this approach for a quantitative analysis of written discourse in literature, news, and social media texts [59-61]. Matthiessen’s [62] work adds valuable insights into the use of SFL in health care contexts and PCC. Pounds and De Pablos-Ortega [63] foresee the combination of the empathy appraisal approach and SFL categories to better understand patients’ (or users’) perspectives and to improve doctors’ (or experts’) communicative strategies in online counseling. Additionally, the experiential metafunction, which is embedded in the ideational metafunction, describes how the speaker uses language to communicate their perception of themselves and the world. For example, Fosgerau et al [64] examined the choices of patients with depression in the transitivity system. Furthermore, this system is the most basic SFL grouping used to quantify the experiential meaning expressed in a text message–based interaction systematically. Thus, a combination of the empathic qualities’ identification in a message and its functional grammar analysis may provide a start point for coding asynchronous messages. Results from this approach would subsequently lead to the identification of an appropriate coaching response.

Thereby, in this paper, we seek to explore whether Pounds’ empathy appraisal and SFL approaches have utility in coding asynchronous text messages in a health context. To that end, we conduct a pilot study to analyze a data set of messages posted by users of a telehealth coaching program. We then discuss how the findings may be used to inform optimal coaching responses to those messages.


Study Design

We coded a sample of 148 messages sent by 29 women from March 7 to June 21, 2021, on a telehealth coaching platform. The study population was an Irish cluster that belongs to an ongoing randomized trial on a telehealth coaching intervention for the prevention of gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM) and postnatal weight loss in 800 women in Australia, Ireland, the United Kingdom, and Spain (Bump2Baby and Me, protocol registration no. ACTRN12620001240932) [65]. The analyzed messages were the first 148 messages sent by the first 29 participants enrolled in the study, who had thus been in the intervention for a period between 0 and 15 weeks. Participants (users) were onboarded after a synchronous initial consultation with their health coach, and then, they received an average of 15 automated messages between enrolment and birth, which included educational material on lifestyle, well-being, and nutrition. Users also received nonautomated messages from their coach, which accounted for an average of 4 weekly, 4 biweekly, and 3 monthly tailored messages before birth. Coach messages included comments on the users’ progress and lifestyle goals, as well as providing educational content and counseling. These communications were based on a predefined structure and a framework grounded on the BCT taxonomy [28] and the motivational interviewing approach [66]. We imported these 148 user-sent messages to NVivo 12 Plus (QSR International), a qualitative analysis software program [67], and then coded them according to the empathy appraisal [53] and SFL [54] categories explained later. Author ERV performed 2 rounds of the coding process for all categories and discussed the issues with a second coder (author HSP).

Ethical Considerations

The Bump2Baby and Me trial, where the authors are authorized researchers, is the source of the data set that was examined. Ethical approval was obtained, and all study participants provided written informed consent for the use of their data for research purposes, provided the findings were presented anonymously. Ethical approval was granted for all study sites (Dublin: National Maternity Hospital Ethics Committee ref EC18.2020; Bristol: Wales Research Ethics Committee ref 21/WA/0022; Granada: CEIM/CEI Provincial de Granada; Melbourne: Monash Health Human Research Ethics Committee ref RES-20-0000-892A). The data used in the study belongs only to the Irish arm of the study (Dublin). More information concerning these ethical considerations can be found in the published study protocol [65].

Empathy Appraisal Categories

We assessed empathy according to the “appraisal” dimensions of empathy in doctor-patient interactions described by Pounds [53], where patients’ expression of feelings and views are categorized as the following EOs:

  • Explicit expressions of negative feelings, such as an emotive behavior or a mental state (“I cried when I found out”).
  • Implicit expression of negative feelings through reference to a negative experience, such as fear, confusion, anxiety, or sadness (“It’s been 3 days and I haven’t heard back from my GP”).
  • Explicit expression of negative judgment (others or self; “She is such an irresponsible person”).
  • Implicit expression of negative judgment (others or self; “I could have done better”).
  • Explicit or implicit expression of positive self-judgment (“I am eating healthier than ever!”).
  • Explicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions; “The dinner was so boring”).
  • Implicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions; “I am not sure this is something for me”).

Message Structure

We used a message structure to explore how each message was organized and whether it affected participants’ expressions of empathy. The main categories were opening, content, and closing, according to previous research on written messaging dynamics [68,69] to illustrate the text structure or “reading path” [70,71]. During the analysis, we created 2 more categories: full structure (Textbox 1), for messages using all 3 categories, and single structure (Textbox 2), for messages using only 1 of them.

Full-structure examples.

Example 1

Opening: “Hi (coach name), hope you're well. Quick question for you.

Content: I weigh myself every Monday morning for the study and I've actually lost weight over the last few weeks. Just 0.15kg. Should I be worried as I read from 15 weeks I should be putting on a pound a week!

Closing: Thanks a mill!”

Example 2

Opening: “Hi (coach name) hope your week is going well (emoji)

Content: so far mine is. Nausea has eased big time in the past 10 which is great and I’ve been having my evening meal. Still need to work on time out for a book etc (emoji) a work in progress. It would be great if you could send me some stretching to over the next few weeks to try keep the body somewhat limber. Find my hips can be a bit creeky or sore in the morning so maybe something to assist?

Closing: Thank you (participant name)”

Textbox 1. Full-structure examples.
Single-structure examples.

Examples

  • “I think I would like to re configure my goals regarding exercise. If I could measure my steps that would probably be a good start to keep tabs on myself? What do you think?”
  • “Thank you very much for all the information (coach name)”
  • “Pilates starts this eve with elbowroom (emoji)” (attached image)
Textbox 2. Single-structure examples.

Sentence Structure

We based the sentence structure categories on Halliday’s [54] systemic functional grammar according to speech function. In a declarative sentence, the subject comes before the finite (verb). In an interrogative sentence, the finite comes before the subject. Lastly, the subject is implicit in an imperative sentence [54]. Throughout the analysis, we found that some sentences had the speech function of a question realized by a declarative structure. Halliday [54] previously described this phenomenon regarding the relationship between the sentence structure and the 4 speech functions offer, command, statement, and question. As a result, we created a fourth category to account for it (Table 1).

Table 1. Sentence structure categories and examples.
Sentence structure categoryExample
Declarative“I signed up for a 4 week yoga class”
Interrogative“What do you think?”
Imperative“Please send them to me”
Declarative structure, question function“I would like to check with you whether you have got any video of pelvic floor exercises”

Processes

We used Halliday’s [54] classification to define the process categories. A process is realized by the verb and contributes to the speaker’s construal of experience. We coded each clause according to the material, mental, relational, behavioral, verbal, and existential process categories (Table 2): Material processes construe the actions of doing and happening. Mental processes account for sensing. Relational processes are used to characterize and identify. Behavioral processes represent outer manifestations of human inner workings, such as consciousness and physiological processes. Verbal processes refer to the language form and use, such as saying and meaning. Lastly, existential processes represent existence or happening [54].

Table 2. Process categories and examples.
Process categoryExample
Material
  • “We made a pumpkin cake”
Mental
  • “I have just read your book”
  • “She is considering your offer”
Relational
  • “The weather was very nice”
  • “I have a blue coat”
Behavioral
  • “I will have a look”
Verbal
  • “We talked about the meeting”
Existential
  • “There is a shop around the corner”

Transitivity Analysis

Halliday’s [72] concept of transitivity supplements the differentiation between transitive and nontransitive verbs. This differentiation depends on the presence or absence of an object that completes the process meaning [72]. Through the choices in the transitivity system, the speaker construes their experiences of the external world and the internal world of their consciousness. This system considers the participants involved, as well as the surrounding circumstances [73]. Thereby, transitivity allowed us to explore the construals of experience in the corpus by identifying the processes and participants [74].


Participants’ Demographics and Program Details

Table 3 shows a description of the users’ demographics and program details. The mean age was 37.59 years (SD 3.69), and the BMI was overall normal (mean 25.82, SD 5.68). Regarding the telehealth coaching program details, users had been on the program for a mean of 80.76 days (SD 30.47) and had sent a mean of 2.27 messages (SD 1.19) at the time of our analysis. In contrast, coaches had sent a mean of 7.62 messages (SD 1.82). The most common goals set by the users were related to physical activity (n=27, 93.1%), diet (n=24, 82.8%), and the number of steps (n=21, 72.4%). Because this was a coaching program for pregnant women, weight was not a frequent goal (n=7, 24.1%) and coaches were encouraged not to promote it. Users could manually add any lifestyle-related goal into the life goals category (n=15, 51.7%) on the platform, such as “meditate in the morning,” “go to bed before midnight,” or “read a book for 20mins.”

We present an overview of the coding results in Table 4 as the number of coded references (occurrences), word count, and word count percentages. We identified 164 EOs, accounting for 42.37% (3478/8209) of the corpus. Negative empathic statements were the most present (n=2026, 24.68%), mostly through an implicit approach (n=1442, 17.57%) as an implicit negative appreciation of things, events, or actions (n=987, 12.02%). This implicit approach rate was similar to that of the explicit or implicit expression of positive self-judgment category (n=1481, 18.04%). We did not identify any explicit expression of negative judgment about others or self.

Content was the predominant structural component (n=7077, 86.21%). Nearly half of the messages (n=4011, 48.86%) included all 3 structural components (opening, content, and closing), while 5.26% (n=432) were identified as a single message, including 1 of the components (opening, content, or closing). In some cases, a user sent more than 1 message at the same time, resulting in the structural components being divided. We conducted a separate analysis comparing full- and single-structured messages that showed no differences for EOs, sentence structure, and processes.

Sentence structure coding revealed that the preferred sentence structure was declarative in this corpus (n=7084, 86.30%). This indicated that participants used these message exchanges to narrate, describe, or state rather than to request for information, guidance, or support. However, 9.43% (774/8209) of the corpus was not coded, because it did not meet the definition of a clause as previously explained (greetings, thanking, laughter, emojis, links in between independent sentences, vocatives), and was labeled as “other.”

Further, processes accounted for 22.40% (n=1839) of the corpus and were material in almost half of the cases (n=876, 10.67%), followed by relational (n=495, 6.03%). In Table 5, we show the process occurrences in percentages (%) for each process category identified in the EOs expressed. Overall, all the processes were evenly spread in both positive and negative EO categories. Material (n=224, 43.2%) and relational (n=192, 37.0%) processes were the most recurrent for expressing EOs, often combined in the same EO category. Participants used material and relational processes similarly to express an explicit negative EO (n=101, 45.1%, and n=81, 42.2%, respectively), mostly for explicit expression of negative appreciation (eg “I was working [material] in the office last week and my diet was [relational] terrible”; n=69, 30.8%, and n=54, 28.1%, respectively) and explicit or implicit expression of positive self-judgement (eg, “On Friday I did [material] my Pilates classes and it was [relational] great after, as a miracle my back pain disappeared [material]”; n=101, 45.1%, and n=79, 41.2%, respectively). In both cases, participants introduced the situation with the material process and communicated their emotions about it with the relational process. In addition, mental and behavioral processes were used more often for negative (eg, “I forgot [mental] to take my multivitamin for 3 days last week” and “Things aren’t the same since before childbirth sometimes when I sneeze [behavioral]”; n=36, 62%, vs n=22, 28%, and n=16, 64%, vs n=9, 36%, respectively) than for positive EOs (eg, “I feel [mental] my sleep is getting better but I think [mental] that might be due to increasing my walking distance” and “…also listening [behavioral] to my body when I need rest and a cup of tea”). Existential (eg, “…however there has been [existential] a day or two were I didnt snack and that reflected in my energy levels and mood”) and verbal processes (eg “I have to admit [verbal] that our portion sizes would be much larger than these”) were marginally identified in 6 (1%) and 14 (3%) of all EOs in a similar proportion for negative and positive expressions (n=3, 50%, each and n=7, 50%, each, respectively). However, when the expressions were negative, participants only used these processes for the explicit expression of negative appreciation category.

Table 3. Participants’ demographics and program details.
Characteristic/detailParticipants
Age (years), mean (SD)37.59 (3.69)
BMI, mean (SD)25.82 (5.68)
Program details, mean (SD)

Days on program80.76 (30.47)

Coach sent messages7.62 (1.82)

User sent messages2.27 (1.19)
Goals of participants, n (%)

Weight7 (24.1)

Physical activity27 (93.1)

Number of steps21 (72.4)

Diet24 (82.8)

Life15 (51.7)
Table 4. Coding results expressed in number of occurrences, word count, and percentage of total word count.
CategoryOccurrences, n (%)Word count (N=8209), n (%)
EOsa164 (100)3478 (42.37)

Implicit expression of negative feelings12 (7.32)351 (4.28)

Implicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions)39 (23.78)987 (12.02)

Implicit expression of negative judgement (others or self)4 (2.44)104 (1.27)

Pooled implicit negative EOs55 (33.54)1442 (17.57)

Explicit expression of negative feelings15 (9.15)267 (3.25)

Explicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions)20 (12.20)317 (3.86)

Explicit expression of negative judgement (others or self)00

Pooled explicit negative EOs35 (21.34)584 (7.11)

Pooled negative EOs90 (54.88)2026 (24.68)

Explicit or implicit expression of positive self-judgement74 (45.12)1481 (18.04)
Message structure (n=148 messages)

Opening75 (34.40)430 (5.24)

Content96 (44.04)7077 (86.21)

Closing47 (21.56)270 (3.29)

Full structure (pooled opening, content, and closing)38 (25.68)4011 (48.86)

Single (opening, content, or closing)28 (18.92)432 (5.26)
Sentence structure (n=734 sentences)

Declarative697 (94.96)7084 (86.30)

Declarative, question function4 (0.54)76 (0.93)

Imperative14 (1.91)88 (1.07)

Interrogative19 (2.59)187 (2.28)

Other0774 (9.43)
Process1025 (100)1839 (22.40)

Behavioral34 (3.32)87 (1.06)

Existential10 (0.98)24 (0.29)

Material430 (41.95)876 (10.67)

Mental180 (17.56)287 (3.50)

Relational325 (31.71)495 (6.03)

Verbal46 (4.49)85 (1.04)

aEO: empathy opportunity.

Table 5. Percentage (%) of occurrences per process category identified for each EOa category.
Process (n=519 occurrences, 50.63%) found in the identified EOs (n=164)Behavioral (n=25, 4.8%), n (%)Existential (n=6, 1.2%), n (%)Material (n=224, 43.2%), n (%)Mental (n=58, 11.2%), n (%)Relational (n= 192, 37.0%), n (%)Verbal (n=14, 2.7%), n (%)
EOs (n=164 occurrences)

Explicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions)3 (12.0)3 (50.0)69 (30.8)13 (22.4)54 (28.1)6 (43.0)

Explicit expression of negative judgement (others or self)2 (8)07 (3.1)2 (3.4)8 (4.2)1 (7.0)

Explicit expressions of negative feelings4 (16.0)025 (11.2)7 (12.1)19 (9.9)0

Pooled explicit negative EOs9 (36.0)3 (50.0)101 (45.1)21 (36.2)81 (42.2)7 (50.0)

Implicit expression of negative appreciation (things, events, actions)5 (20.0)016 (7.1)3 (5.2)17 (8.9)0

Implicit expression of negative judgement (others or self)000000

Implicit expressions of negative feelings2 (8.0)07 (3.1)11 (19.0)15 (7.8)0

Pooled implicit negative EOs7 (28.0)022 (9.8)15 (25.9)33 (17.2)0

Pooled negative EOs16 (64.0)3 (50.0)123 (54.9)36 (62.1)113 (58.9)7 (50.0)

Explicit or implicit expression of positive self-judgement9 (36.0)3 (50.0)101 (45.1)22 (37.9)79 (41.1)7 (50.0)

aEO: empathy opportunity.

Transitivity Analysis

When we performed a transitivity analysis, the participant roles varied according to the process type. As shown in Table 4, material processes dominated the text corpus (n=876, 10.67%), followed by relational (n=495, 6.03%). Relational processes are used for either characterizing, including a carrier and an attribute as components of the system, or identifying, involving a value and a token. Material processes, on the other hand, include an actor (participant), and some demand a goal, while others do not [54]. In addition to these grammatical roles, we categorized findings from this analysis thematically to supplement the meanings expressed. We present the results from transitivity and thematic analyses in Tables 6-11. Most (n=300, 92.2%) relational processes were attributive (eg, “Your links were very helpful”). The remaining 7.8% (n=25) were identifying (“My starting weight was 51.5kg”). The most frequent themes were food and diet (n=63, 19.3%), well-being (n=60, 18.2%), and physical activity (n=44, 13.5%). Similarly, material processes frequently (n=217, 70.4%) had the user as the actor, and although their goals were widely spread, the most common categories were food and diet (n=196, 54.9%), physical activity (n=96, 26.9%), and goals (n=40, 11.2%). For example, “I open the dates put a bit of peanut butter in them, then put them in the freezer to harden” and “I've added a pelvic floor exercise goal.” In contrast, mental processes (n=287, 3.5%) involve a senser and a phenomenon in the transitivity system. This corpus showed a predominance (n=84, 93.3%) of the user as the senser and food and diet (n=24, 26.1%), well-being (n=16, 18.9%) and physical activity (n=13, 14.8%) as the phenomenon (eg, “I have included new snacks like olives” and “I decided to have a go with cross trainer”). In verbal clauses, a sayer directs a message to a receiver. In this corpus, despite its low occurrence (n=85, 1.0%), the most frequent sayer was the user (n=19, 55.9%) and the receiver was usually a health care professional (HCP; n=5, 38.5%; eg, “I talked with my GP about the pains”). The most common thematic, as in the other processes, was food and diet (n=9, 20.5%), with an identical occurrence to well-being (n=9, 20.5%).

Table 6. Transitivity analysis results for material processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Actor (n=308)

User217 (70.4)

Not human63 (20.7)

We11 (3.6)

Another person6 (1.8)

Coach5 (1.6)

User’s HCPa4 (1.3)

User’s partner2 (0.6)
Goal (n=357)

Food and diet196 (54.9)

Physical activity96 (26.9)

Goals40 (11.2)

Other (something, nothing, anything, things)17 (4.6)

Other (place, object, pain, work, mood, medicine, body part)5 (1.4)

Message (user or coach sent)2 (0.5)

Person (user, coach, baby, HCP)1 (0.2)

App1 (0.2)

aHCP: health care professional.

Table 7. Transitivity analysis results for relational processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Attributive300 (92.2)

With a carrier266 (81.7)

Without a carrier34 (10.4)
Identifying25 (7.8)
Themes (n=325)

Food and diet63 (19.3)

Well-being60 (18.2)

Physical activity44 (13.5)

Goals30 (9.1)

Pregnancy and baby27 (8.1)

Pain25 (7.8)

Stress21 (6.4)

Work16 (4.7)

App14 (4.4)

Coach messages9 (2.7)

Mood and emotions8 (2.4)

User messages5 (1.4)

Weather3 (1.0)

App1 (0.2)
Table 8. Transitivity analysis results for mental processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Senser (n=90)

User84 (93.3)

Coach3 (3.2)

Doctor2 (2.1)

User’s partner1 (1.1)
Phenomenon (n=90)

Food and diet24 (26.1)

Well-being16 (18.2)

Physical activity13 (14.8)

Goals9 (10.2)

App9 (10.2)

Pain9 (10.2)

Planning4 (4.6)

Baby3 (3.4)

Coach messages2 (2.3)
Table 9. Transitivity analysis results for behavioral processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Behaver (n=15)

User13 (94.3)

You1 (2.9)

We1 (2.9)
Themes (n=35)

Food and diet13 (38.2)

Physical activity5 (14.7)

Pain3 (8.8)

App3 (8.8)

Goals3 (8.8)

Pregnancy and baby3 (8.8)

Well-being3 (8.8)

Coach messages1 (2.9)
Table 10. Transitivity analysis results for verbal processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Sayer (n=34)

User19 (55.9)

Coach12 (35.3)

HCPa2 (5.9)

We1 (2.9)
Receiver (n=13)

HCP5 (38.5)

User4 (30.8)

Coach4 (30.8)
Themes (n=46)

Food and diet9 (20.5)

Well-being9 (20.5)

Coach messages7 (15.4)

Pain6 (12.8)

Physical activity6 (12.8)

Goals4 (7.7)

User messages2 (5.1)

App1 (2.6)

Pregnancy and baby1 (2.6)

aHCP: health care professional.

Table 11. Transitivity analysis results for existential processes.
Processes, grammatical roles, and themesOccurrences, n (%)
Themes (n=10)

Physical activity3 (30.0)

Food and diet3 (30.0)

Stress2 (20.0)

App1 (10.0)

Pregnancy and baby1 (10.0)

Principal Findings

Empathy and SFL approaches, as we hypothesized in the introduction, can be successfully combined. Our findings show that the SFL categories we explored in the transitivity analysis correspond to and supplement Pounds' EOs. Our findings reveal interesting meanings that originate from the user's linguistic choices whenever they express an EO, particularly when these are “hidden” in an implicit form. Since HCPs frequently overlook these in patient-provider communication, identifying and responding to them optimally is critical for successful health interventions. To the best of our knowledge, no other researchers have previously presented this novel perspective.

Overall, our results show that the users expressed negative EOs more often than positive ones (74 vs 90), of which 60% (55 of 90) were expressed implicitly. Given the context of our data set—a coaching program in which pregnant women communicate with a coach who supports them throughout their journey—the existence of negative EOs is not surprising. We frequently use negative statements to draw attention to a problem that we expect the receiver to empathize with or assist us with. Positive EOs, on the other hand, are less common because they do not serve the purpose of seeking support. However, they do provide a chance for the coach to praise the user’s behavior [53]. Moreover, the user’s preference for implicit EOs could be due to a polite relationship with their coaches, which would prevent them from making too negative statements. The absence of explicit expressions of negative judgment about others or self could support this interpretation. Such insights could be useful for coaches to detect empathic expressions and support users further. Moreover, our results from the message and sentence structure analyses indicated that most of the wording used was found in the content section of the message with a predominant use of a declarative structure. We explored a relationship between the empathy categories and the message structure, and between the empathy categories and the sentence structure. Such analyses showed no variability in the data across categories; hence, we chose not to include them in this paper. Nevertheless, the predominance of this sentence structure finding is expected, as the use of statements prevails in lengthier and more monological, narrational stretches of communication, which most of these messages were. The scarce presence of interrogative sentences (2.28%) shows that these users were not posing questions and asking for help. Nonetheless, a more qualitative analysis of interrogative occurrences could provide a deeper understanding of these linguistic choices. We added a fourth category during our analysis to account for those sentences where a question (interrogative function) was atypically realized through a statement (declarative structure), representing 0.93% of the sentences in the corpus. A functional interpretation of this phenomenon could be that users were moderating their queries to be less imposing and less direct. The short interaction time (3 months) can also explain this declarative choice, indicating an insufficient time allowed to develop a coach-user relationship. At the start of the program, users met their coach during a synchronous call followed by a small number of purely asynchronous interactions (the mean for coach-sent messages was 7.62 and user-sent messages was 2.27). Participants could be afraid or not feel the confidence to actively ask for information or help due to an insufficient or too polite relationship with their coach. Another reason could be low trust on or missing information about the coach’s ability to support them.

Our transitivity analysis showed that the users were the main participants in the clause (eg, material processes described their lifestyle actions, such as food and diet, physical activity, and goals), such as cooking, eating, or exercising, as this was a lifestyle, goal setting–based coaching program. Additionally, these results were in line with the characteristics of the EOs detected: material processes were predominately used for expressions of positive self-judgment (eg, “I am eating healthy and doing long walks every day”) or implicit expressions of negative appreciation (eg, “I only had one Panadol for pain management, but it does not work that well”); mental processes disclosed negative feelings (eg, “I feel extremely tired and struggling to get 10 thousand step per day”), and negative appreciations were realized through attributive, relational processes (eg, “my snacking has been desperate”). These are highly interesting findings for health behavior change programs that have the potential to contribute to promoting user outcomes [75]. The information shared by the user, being explicit or implicit, helps the coach understand the user’s perspectives and coaching needs. Although explicit expressions are easy to detect, a more efficient detection and understanding of implicit expressions will contribute to better coaching support. Our insights will provide guidance on the most empathic coach responses and serve to determine the optimal text message–based coach responses in telehealth interventions. Because of its relationship to the EO categories, transitivity analysis opens a range of opportunities for improving patient care overall. Our perspectives for these findings include further exploration of the empathy and the linguistic elements (SFL) found in the coaches’ optimal responses and their connection with user outcomes. Moreover, an association with the BCTs used by the coaches in response to these messages could provide additional insights to boost the impact of digital lifestyle text-based interventions.

Comparison With Prior Work

As we previously described in the Introduction section of this paper, prior work has mainly studied empathy and linguistics in text-based communication separately. Empathy is an important element in the patient-provider relationship that improves patient outcomes [47]. Some authors have measured empathy in a digital setting with surveys [76], with different indexes or scales [77], or as a predefined element in broader coding systems [78]. Other research work has focused on a computational approach to automatize empathy detection (eg, in digital mental health services [79,80]). With regard to linguistics, there has been an increasing interest in digital communication [81], and researchers have applied different linguistic perspectives, such as digital conversation analysis (CA) [82] and SFL. Both CA and SFL perspectives have been applied to digital contexts, such as social media and digital consultations [83]. However, Pounds [53] was the first one to recently define and demonstrate the use of empathy appraisal categories in text-based, patient-provider interactions. The appraisal framework generally studies the meaning negotiation among the speakers, using every utterance to align or misalign with others. In SFL, this framework describes the linguistic resources that the speakers use to construe their social experience and build an intersubjectivity with the recipient, contributing to the interpersonal metafunction [84]. Furthermore, according to Martin and White [85], the appraisal system is organized in 3 domains used to negotiate and modulate emotions, judgements, and valuations: engagement, attitude, and graduation. The attitude is the system of meanings represented by the feelings expressed. Graduation intensifies or diminishes this representation of meanings. Engagement reflects the commitment of the speaker to the appraisal expressed. These appraisal system domains are further explained by Martin [86] as an expansion of the theoretical and descriptive focus of SFL described by Halliday and serve to analyze the speakers’ feelings. Pounds’ empathy appraisal categories are grounded on this research work and were later suggested for their combination with SFL categories by Pounds herself and De-Pablos Ortega [42]. We contacted Pounds and De-Pablos Ortega for research collaboration. However, they confirmed that they had discontinued their work on the topic. Thereby, we are the first to code a text-based health interaction using both an empathy and an SFL approach. This pilot study served to assess whether these 2 methodologies were compatible, with promising results. We will include further features previously used in text-message coding, such as sentiment analysis, in our future research. Some software can perform automatic sentiment analysis (eg, through word rating [87]), while other authors have resorted to more elaborate machine learning and algorithms for more accurate results [88,89]. Furthermore, coding for additional elements (eg, emojis or modality) could inform the negotiation of interpersonal roles, such as the user-coach relationship, and be paired with the EO displayed [73].

Limitations

Given the small sample size (n=148), our results should be carefully observed. We aimed to test the combination of empathy and linguistic approaches for text messages analysis. Our findings are preliminary and part of a broader project that will continue exploring methodological possibilities in asynchronous communication analysis.

Conclusion

Our transitivity analysis supports the combination of an empathy and a linguistic (SFL) approach. The processes and their related elements correlate with the empathy categories identified in the corpus. These are promising results for future coding in asynchronous, online interactions. Our study findings shed light on the empathy and linguistic characteristics present in text message–based coaching. We draw attention to the meanings of patient EOs, such as implicitly seeking help or praise, because research shows that HCPs frequently miss these opportunities. Their identification and management have significant implications for the coach-user relationship and to improve coach training in the future. Our next steps will be to study the coaches’ messages and to explore the coach-user relationship-building process. We will code the coaches’ messages for linguistic choices (SFL) and how they respond to the EOs presented by the users. Additionally, we will link our results with user outcomes in this lifestyle coaching program during pregnancy when at risk of GDM. This future research will allow for the formulation of optimal coach empathic responses.

Acknowledgments

ERV performed data collection and analysis, synthesized the results, and prepared and edited the manuscript until submission. HSP contributed to the data analysis and interpretation processes, provided reliability coding, and participated in the manuscript reviewing process. TS conceived the research, supported the interpretation and discussion of results, and reviewed the final manuscript. This research is part of an industrial PhD project sponsored by Innovation Fund Denmark, the University of Copenhagen, and Liva Healthcare. The Impact Diabetes B2B (Bump2Baby and Me) project has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant agreement no. 847984). The project also acknowledges collaborative funding from the National Health and Medical Research Council (grant no. 1194234). We would like to acknowledge the contribution of the members of the Impact Diabetes B2B Collaboration Group: Associate Professor Sharleen L O'Reilly (University College Dublin), Dr Aisling A Geraghty (University College Dublin), Dr Faisal Zahoor (University College Dublin), Prof Fionnuala M McAuliffe (University College Dublin), Associate Professor Mary Codd (University College Dublin), Associate Professor Ricardo Segurado (University College Dublin), Prof Helle T Maindal (Aarhus University), Nanna Husted Jensen (Aarhus University), Dr Anna Davies (University of Bristol), Associate Professor Christy Burden (University of Bristol), Prof Jane E Norman (University of Bristol), Prof Karsten Vrangbæk (University of Copenhagen), Dr Laura Elina Pirhonen (University of Copenhagen), Prof Enrique Herrera-Viedman (University of Granada), Dr Mercedes Bermúdez (University of Granada), Prof Cristina Campoy (University of Granada), Prof Alberto Puertas (University of Granada), Dr Francisca S Molina (University of Granada), Dr Ditte Hjorth Laursen (Liva Healthcare), Katie Angotti (Liva Healthcare), Stig Jørgensen (Liva Healthcare), Prof Karen J Campbell (Deakin University), Associate Professor Rachel Laws (Deakin University), Associate Professor Vincent L Versace (Deakin University), Prof Helena Teede (Monash University), Dr Cheryce L Harrison (Monash University), Associate Professor Jacqueline Boyle (Monash University), Dr Georgia Soldartis (Monash University).

Data Availability

The data sets generated and analyzed during this study are not publicly available due to the presence of sensitive information, such as names and places, in the user messages analyzed. Researchers who wish to access the full data set may be granted access upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

ERV is employed at the company that provides coaching services for the study trial where the research was conducted (Liva Healthcare).

  1. World Health Organization. Global Health Estimates 2020: Deaths by Cause, Age, Sex, by Country and by Region, 2000-2019. 2020.   URL: https:/​/www.​who.int/​data/​gho/​data/​themes/​mortality-and-global-health-estimates/​ghe-leading -causes-of-death [accessed 2022-11-29]
  2. Sakakibara BM, Kim AJ, Eng JJ. A systematic review and meta-analysis on self-management for improving risk factor control in stroke patients. Int J Behav Med 2017 Feb;24(1):42-53 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Alageel S, Gulliford MC, McDermott L, Wright AJ. Multiple health behaviour change interventions for primary prevention of cardiovascular disease in primary care: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open 2017 Jun 15;7(6):e015375 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Almutairi N, Hosseinzadeh H, Gopaldasani V. The effectiveness of patient activation intervention on type 2 diabetes mellitus glycemic control and self-management behaviors: a systematic review of RCTs. Prim Care Diabetes 2020 Mar;14(1):12-20. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Liu Y, Croft JB, Wheaton AG, Kanny D, Cunningham TJ, Lu H, et al. Clustering of five health-related behaviors for chronic disease prevention among adults, United States, 2013. Prev Chronic Dis 2016 May 26;13:E70 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Cradock KA, ÓLaighin G, Finucane FM, Gainforth HL, Quinlan LR, Ginis KAM. Behaviour change techniques targeting both diet and physical activity in type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2017 Mar 08;14(1):18 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Ely EK, Gruss SM, Luman ET, Albright AL. Response to comment on Ely et al. A national effort to prevent type 2 diabetes: participant-level evaluation of CDC's National Diabetes Prevention Program. Diabetes Care 2017;40:1331-1341. Diabetes Care 2017 Nov 23;40(11):e163-e163. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Valabhji J, Barron E, Bradley D, Bakhai C, Fagg J, O'Neill S, et al. Early outcomes from the English National Health Service Diabetes Prevention Programme. Diabetes Care 2020 Jan;43(1):152-160 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Nelson R. Telemedicine and telehealth: the potential to improve rural access to care. Am J Nurs 2017 Jun;117(6):17-18. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Speyer R, Denman D, Wilkes-Gillan S, Chen Y, Bogaardt H, Kim J, et al. Effects of telehealth by allied health professionals and nurses in rural and remote areas: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Rehabil Med 2018 Mar 28;50(3):225-235 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Wu C, Wu Z, Yang L, Zhu W, Zhang M, Zhu Q, et al. Evaluation of the clinical outcomes of telehealth for managing diabetes: a PRISMA-compliant meta-analysis. Medicine (Baltimore) 2018 Oct;97(43):e12962 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  12. Berry RM, Ashby JS, Gnilka PB, Matheny KB. A comparison of face-to-face and distance coaching practices: coaches' perceptions of the role of the working alliance in problem resolution. Consult Psychol J: Pract Res 2011 Dec;63(4):243-253. [CrossRef]
  13. Shigekawa E, Fix M, Corbett G, Roby DH, Coffman J. The current state of telehealth evidence: a rapid review. Health Aff (Millwood) 2018 Dec;37(12):1975-1982. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. An S, Song R. Effects of health coaching on behavioral modification among adults with cardiovascular risk factors: systematic review and meta-analysis. Patient Educ Couns 2020 Oct;103(10):2029-2038 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Nelson LA, Mayberry LS, Wallston K, Kripalani S, Bergner EM, Osborn CY. Development and usability of REACH: a tailored theory-based text messaging intervention for disadvantaged adults with type 2 diabetes. JMIR Hum Factors 2016 Sep 08;3(2):e23 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Malecki HL, Gollie JM, Scholten J. Physical activity, exercise, whole health, and integrative health coaching. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 2020 Nov;31(4):649-663. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Bodenheimer T. Coaching patients to be active, informed partners in their health. Fam Syst Health 2020 Jun;38(2):190-192. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Thiyagarajan A, Grant C, Griffiths F, Atherton H. Exploring patients' and clinicians' experiences of video consultations in primary care: a systematic scoping review. BJGP Open 2020;4(1):bjgpopen20X101020 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Seppen BF, den Boer P, Wiegel J, Ter Wee MM, van der Leeden M, de Vries R, et al. Asynchronous mHealth interventions in rheumatoid arthritis: systematic scoping review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2020 Nov 05;8(11):e19260 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Linden A, Butterworth S, Prochaska J. Motivational interviewing-based health coaching as a chronic care intervention. J Eval Clin Pract 2010 Mar;16(1):166-174. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. Klein M, Mogles N, van Wissen A. Intelligent mobile support for therapy adherence and behavior change. J Biomed Inform 2014 Oct;51:137-151 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Hansel B, Giral P, Gambotti L, Lafourcade A, Peres G, Filipecki C, et al. A fully automated web-based program improves lifestyle habits and HbA1c in patients with type 2 diabetes and abdominal obesity: randomized trial of patient e-coaching nutritional support (the ANODE Study). J Med Internet Res 2017 Nov 08;19(11):e360 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Wang L, Fagan C, Yu C. Popular mental health apps (MH apps) as a complement to telepsychotherapy: guidelines for consideration. J Psychother Integr 2020 Jun;30(2):265-273. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. White JS, Toussaert S, Thrul J, Bontemps-Jones J, Abroms L, Westmaas JL. Peer mentoring and automated text messages for smoking cessation: a randomized pilot trial. Nicotine Tob Res 2020 Mar 16;22(3):371-380. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Damschroder LJ, Buis LR, McCant FA, Kim HM, Evans R, Oddone EZ, et al. Effect of adding telephone-based brief coaching to an mHealth app (Stay Strong) for promoting physical activity among veterans: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res 2020 Aug 04;22(8):e19216 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Singh HK, Kennedy GA, Stupans I. Does the modality used in health coaching matter? A systematic review of health coaching outcomes. Patient Prefer Adherence 2020 Aug;14:1477-1492 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Berry MP, Sala M, Abber SR, Forman EM. Incorporating automated digital interventions into coach-delivered weight loss treatment: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol 2021 Aug;40(8):534-545. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Michie S, Richardson M, Johnston M, Abraham C, Francis J, Hardeman W, et al. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med 2013 Aug;46(1):81-95 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Gillison FB, Rouse P, Standage M, Sebire SJ, Ryan RM. A meta-analysis of techniques to promote motivation for health behaviour change from a self-determination theory perspective. Health Psychol Rev 2019 Mar;13(1):110-130. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Pourrazavi S, Kouzekanani K, Bazargan-Hejazi S, Shaghaghi A, Hashemiparast M, Fathifar Z, et al. Theory-based e-health literacy interventions in older adults: a systematic review. Arch Public Health 2020 Aug 10;78(1):72 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Sineath A, Lambert L, Verga C, Wagstaff M, Wingo BC. Monitoring intervention fidelity of a lifestyle behavioral intervention delivered through telehealth. Mhealth 2017 Aug 25;3:35 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Zengul A, Evans E, Hall A, Qu H, Willig A, Cherrington A, et al. Telehealth behavioral intervention for diabetes management in adults with physical disabilities: intervention fidelity protocol for a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res Protoc 2021 Sep 10;10(9):e31695 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Dogar O, Boehnke JR, Lorencatto F, Sheldon TA, Siddiqi K. Measuring fidelity to behavioural support delivery for smoking cessation and its association with outcomes. Addiction 2020 Feb;115(2):368-380 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Hurlocker MC, Madson MB, Schumacher JA. Motivational interviewing quality assurance: a systematic review of assessment tools across research contexts. Clin Psychol Rev 2020 Dec;82:101909 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Chai LK, Collins CE, May C, Brown LJ, Ashman A, Burrows TL. Fidelity and acceptability of a family-focused technology-based telehealth nutrition intervention for child weight management. J Telemed Telecare 2021 Mar;27(2):98-109. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Salloum RG, Rojewski AM, Piper ME, Blalock JA, Borrelli B, Boyce LM, et al. Reporting treatment fidelity in behavioral tobacco treatment clinical trials: scoping review and measurement recommendations. Nicotine Tob Res 2022 Mar 01;24(2):150-159 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Suffoletto B. Text message behavioral interventions: from here to where? Curr Opin Psychol 2016 Mar 01;9:16-21 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Chow CK, Redfern J, Hillis GS, Thakkar J, Santo K, Hackett ML, et al. Effect of lifestyle-focused text messaging on risk factor modification in patients with coronary heart disease: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 2015;314(12):1255-1263. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Paulus T, Warren A, Lester J. Applying conversation analysis methods to online talk: a literature review. Discourse, Context Media 2016 Jun;12:1-10 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  40. Villarreal A. Constructing Meaning: A Systemic Functional Review of Translated Health Texts and Community Narratives. Ann Arbor, MI: ProQuest; 2017.
  41. Irvine A, Drew P, Bower P, Brooks H, Gellatly J, Armitage CJ, et al. Are there interactional differences between telephone and face-to-face psychological therapy? A systematic review of comparative studies. J Affect Disord 2020 Mar 15;265:120-131 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Pounds G, De Pablos -Ortega C. Patient-centred communication in British, Italian and Spanish ‘Ask-the-Expert’ healthcare websites. Commun Med 2015 Nov 07;12(2-3):225-241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Jeffrey D. Empathy, sympathy and compassion in healthcare: is there a problem? Is there a difference? Does it matter? J R Soc Med 2016 Dec 06;109(12):446-452 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Chochinov HM. Dignity and the essence of medicine: the A, B, C, and D of dignity conserving care. BMJ 2007 Jul 28;335(7612):184-187 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Guidi C, Traversa C. Empathy in patient care: from 'clinical empathy' to 'empathic concern'. Med Health Care Philos 2021 Dec 01;24(4):573-585 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Piasecki M. Clinical Communication Handbook. Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Science; 2003.
  47. Howick J, Moscrop A, Mebius A, Fanshawe TR, Lewith G, Bishop FL, et al. Effects of empathic and positive communication in healthcare consultations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J R Soc Med 2018 Jul;111(7):240-252 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Bonvicini KA, Perlin MJ, Bylund CL, Carroll G, Rouse RA, Goldstein MG. Impact of communication training on physician expression of empathy in patient encounters. Patient Educ Couns 2009 Apr;75(1):3-10 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Bylund CL, Makoul G. Examining empathy in medical encounters: an observational study using the Empathic Communication Coding System. Health Commun 2005 Oct;18(2):123-140. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Hsu I, Saha S, Korthuis PT, Sharp V, Cohn J, Moore RD, et al. Providing support to patients in emotional encounters: a new perspective on missed empathic opportunities. Patient Educ Couns 2012 Sep;88(3):436-442 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  51. Muñoz-Miquel A. Empathy, emotions and patient-centredness: a case study on communication strategies. HJLCB 2019 Nov 01;59(1):71-89. [CrossRef]
  52. Epstein RM, Franks P, Fiscella K, Shields CG, Meldrum SC, Kravitz RL, et al. Measuring patient-centered communication in patient-physician consultations: theoretical and practical issues. Soc Sci Med 2005 Oct;61(7):1516-1528. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Pounds G. Empathy as ‘appraisal’: a new language-based approach to the exploration of clinical empathy. J Appl Linguistics Prof Pract 2010 Dec 22;7(2):145-168. [CrossRef]
  54. Halliday M, Matthiessen CMIM. Halliday's Introduction to Functional Grammar. 4th ed. London, UK: Routledge; 2014.
  55. Halliday M. Language as Social Semiotic: The Social Interpretation of Language and Meaning. London, UK: Edward Arnold; 1978.
  56. Suchman AL, Markakis K, Beckman HB, Frankel R. A model of empathic communication in the medical interview. JAMA 1997 Feb 26;277(8):678. [CrossRef]
  57. Banks D. Systemic functional linguistics as a model for text analysis. ASp 2002 Dec 01(35-36):23-34. [CrossRef]
  58. Martin J, Matthiessen CMIM, Painter C. Working with Functional Grammar. London, UK: Edward Arnold; 1997.
  59. Zhang Y. Transitivity analysis of Hillary Clinton’s and Donald Trump’s first television debate. Int J Appl Linguist Engl Lit 2017 Oct 10;6(7):65. [CrossRef]
  60. Wahyudin AY. An analysis of process type used in news item. TEKNOSASTIK 2016;14(1):1-8. [CrossRef]
  61. Klaisingto P. A transitivity analysis of the experience of depression as expressed on Thai Twitter. J Liberal Arts 2021;13(1):188-215.
  62. Matthiessen CMIM. Applying systemic functional linguistics in healthcare contexts. Text Talk 2013;33(4-5):437-466. [CrossRef]
  63. Pounds G. Patient-centred communication in ask-the-expert healthcare websites. Appl Linguistics 2018;39(2):117-134. [CrossRef]
  64. Fosgerau CF, Davidsen AS. Patients’ representations of depressive symptoms and physicians’ responses in clinical encounters. CAM 2019 Jul 03;15(1):28-39. [CrossRef]
  65. O'Reilly SL, Burden C, Campoy C, McAuliffe FM, Teede H, Andresen J, IMPACT DIABETES B2B Collaboration Group. Bump2Baby and Me: protocol for a randomised trial of mHealth coaching for healthy gestational weight gain and improved postnatal outcomes in high-risk women and their children. Trials 2021 Dec 28;22(1):963 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  66. Hardcastle SJ, Fortier M, Blake N, Hagger MS. Identifying content-based and relational techniques to change behaviour in motivational interviewing. Health Psychol Rev 2017 Mar;11(1):1-16. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. QSR International. NVivo.   URL: https://www.qsrinternational.com/nvivo-qualitative-data-analysis-software/home [accessed 2022-11-29]
  68. Bernicot J, Volckaert-Legrier O, Goumi A, Bert-Erboul A. Forms and functions of SMS messages: a study of variations in a corpus written by adolescents. J Pragmat 2012 Sep;44(12):1701-1715. [CrossRef]
  69. Herring SC, Androutsopoulos J. Computer-mediated discourse 2.0. In: The Handbook of Discourse Analysis. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 2015.
  70. McHoul AW. Telling how Texts Talk: Essays on Reading and Ethnomethodology. London, UK: Routledge & K. Paul; 1982.
  71. Have PT. Structuring writing for reading: hypertext and the reading body. Hum Stud 1999 Oct;22(2-4):273-298. [CrossRef]
  72. Wang H. The problems of transitivity studies and its solution. JSS 2015;03(10):170-176. [CrossRef]
  73. Halliday MAK, Matthiessen CMIM. An Introduction to Functional Grammar. 3 ed. London, UK: Edward Arnold; 2004.
  74. Morley G. Syntax in Functional Grammar: An Introduction to Lexicogrammar in Systemic Linguistics. London, UK: Continuum International; 2000.
  75. Dennison L, Morrison L, Conway G, Yardley L. Opportunities and challenges for smartphone applications in supporting health behavior change: qualitative study. J Med Internet Res 2013 Apr 18;15(4):e86 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  76. Nambisan P. Information seeking and social support in online health communities: impact on patients' perceived empathy. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2011 May 01;18(3):298-304 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  77. Shen L. On a scale of state empathy during message processing. West J Commun 2010 Oct 29;74(5):504-524. [CrossRef]
  78. Malik SH, Coulson NS. Coping with infertility online: an examination of self-help mechanisms in an online infertility support group. Patient Educ Couns 2010 Nov;81(2):315-318. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  79. Sharma A, Miner A, Atkins D, Althoff T. A computational approach to understanding empathy expressed in text-based mental health support. 2020 Presented at: Proceedings of the 2020 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP); November 2020; Online. [CrossRef]
  80. Yao H, De SA, Foster A, Galynker I, Lok B. Toward automated evaluation of empathetic responses in virtual human interaction systems for mental health scenarios. 2020 Presented at: Proceedings of the 20th ACM International Conference on Intelligent Virtual Agents (IVA 2020); October 20-22, 2020; Virtual. [CrossRef]
  81. Taiwo R, editor. Handbook of Research on Discourse BehaviorDigital Communication: Language StructuresSocial Interaction (2 Volumes). Hershey, PA: IGI Global; 2010.
  82. Virtanen MT, Kääntä L. At the intersection of text and conversation analysis: analysing asynchronous online written interaction. AFinLA Theme 2018 Dec 14(11):137-155. [CrossRef]
  83. Gardner S, Alsop S. Systemic Functional Linguistics in the Digital Age. Sheffield, UK: Equinox; 2016.
  84. Oteíza T. The appraisal framework and discourse analysis. In: The Routledge Handbook of Systemic Functional Linguistics. London, UK: Routledge; 2017.
  85. Martin J, White P. The Language of Evaluation: Appraisal in English. New York, NY: Springer; 2005.
  86. Martin JR. Evolving systemic functional linguistics: beyond the clause. Funct Linguist 2014 Apr 10;1(3):1-24. [CrossRef]
  87. NVIVO. Automatically Detect and Code Sentiment.   URL: https:/​/help-nv.​qsrinternational.com/​20/​win/​Content/​coding/​auto-detect-code-sentiment.​htm?Highlight=sentiment [accessed 2022-11-29]
  88. Medhat W, Hassan A, Korashy H. Sentiment analysis algorithms and applications: a survey. Ain Shams Eng J 2014 Dec;5(4):1093-1113 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  89. Greaves F, Ramirez-Cano D, Millett C, Darzi A, Donaldson L. Use of sentiment analysis for capturing patient experience from free-text comments posted online. J Med Internet Res 2013 Nov 01;15(11):e239 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]


AI: artificial Intelligence
BCT: behavior change technique
CA: conversation analysis
EO: empathy opportunity
GDM: gestational diabetes mellitus
HCP: health care professional
IHC: integrative health coaching
NCD: noncommunicable disease
PCC: patient-centered communication
SFL: systemic functional linguistics


Edited by A Mavragani; submitted 07.06.22; peer-reviewed by Z Demjen, A Holsting; comments to author 02.11.22; revised version received 21.11.22; accepted 21.11.22; published 20.12.22

Copyright

©Elena Rey Velasco, Hanne Sæderup Pedersen, Timothy Skinner, Impact Diabetes B2B Collaboration Group. Originally published in JMIR Formative Research (https://formative.jmir.org), 20.12.2022.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Formative Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://formative.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.