Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Expressive writing and motivational interviewing are well-known approaches to help patients cope with stressful life events. Although these methods are often applied by human counselors, it is less well understood if an automated artificial intelligence approach can benefit patients. Providing an automated method would help expose a wider range of people to the possible benefits of motivational interviewing, with lower cost and more adaptability to sudden events like the COVID-19 pandemic.
Objective: This study presents an automated writing system and evaluates possible outcomes among participants with respect to behavior related to the COVID-19 pandemic.
Methods: We developed a rule-based dialogue system for “Expressive Interviewing” to elicit writing from participants on the subject of how COVID-19 has impacted their lives. The system prompts participants to describe their life experiences and emotions and provides topic-specific prompts in response to participants’ use of topical keywords. In May 2021 and June 2021, we recruited participants (N=151) via Prolific to complete either the Expressive Interviewing task or a control task. We surveyed participants immediately before the intervention, immediately after the intervention, and again 2 weeks after the intervention. We measured participants’ self-reported stress, general mental health, COVID-19–related health behavior, and social behavior.
Results: Participants generally wrote long responses during the task (53.3 words per response). In aggregate, task participants experienced a significant decrease in stress in the short term (~23% decrease, P<.001) and a slight difference in social activity compared with the control group (P=.03). No significant differences in short-term or long-term outcomes were detected between participant subgroups (eg, male versus female participants) except for some within-condition differences by ethnicity (eg, higher social activity among African American people participating in Expressive Interviewing vs participants of other ethnicities). For short-term effects, participants showed different outcomes based on their writing. Using more anxiety-related words was correlated with a greater short-term decrease in stress (r=–0.264, P<.001), and using more positive emotion words was correlated with a more meaningful experience (r=0.243, P=.001). As for long-term effects, writing with more lexical diversity was correlated with an increase in social activity (r=0.266, P<.001).
Conclusions: Expressive Interviewing participants exhibited short-term, but not long-term, positive changes in mental health, and some linguistic metrics of writing style were correlated with positive change in behavior. Although there were no significant long-term effects observed, the positive short-term effects suggest that the Expressive Interviewing intervention could be used in cases in which a patient lacks access to traditional therapy and needs a short-term solution.
Trial Registration: Clincaltrials.gov NCT05949840; https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT05949840
doi:10.2196/40277
Keywords
Introduction
The COVID-19 pandemic has had a range of adverse effects on people across the world, increasing stress and anxiety for many. As of February 2023, the United States had recorded over 100 million COVID-19 infections and 1.1 million deaths. Actions taken by individuals, such as getting vaccinated, getting tested, or wearing a mask, can reduce the spread of the disease. Reducing stress during the pandemic can have important psychosocial and behavioral outcomes, especially for people who feel a loss of control over their lives as a result of pandemic restrictions [
, ].Expressive writing is a behavioral intervention paradigm in which people are encouraged to explore their emotions and thoughts about significant life events. This method has had a positive impact on participants’ physical and mental health [
, ], including a decrease in physician visits, adoption of positive behavior, and improved moods. Along similar lines, motivational interviewing is a counseling technique that leverages a person’s intrinsic motivation and values to help them change their behavior. Motivational interviewing is known to correlate with positive changes for many different types of goals, such as weight management [ ], chronic disease management [ ], and substance use [ ]. Furthermore, applying motivational interviewing in virtual environments has proven effective in encouraging behavior change [ , ].Although such techniques can lead to positive change in participants, they may be inaccessible to people who lack access to the resources or time that the techniques require [
, ]. This problem can compound in situations where a sudden event such as the COVID-19 pandemic overwhelms health care resources and disproportionately deprives vulnerable subpopulations of health care resources [ ]. Recent studies have proposed automated systems to grant more patients access to therapeutic techniques [ , ]. However, current dialogue systems often use generic or irrelevant prompts that do not adapt to participants’ responses, which may result in less engagement and possibly less behavior change from patients [ ]. To address this shortcoming, we evaluated a system that integrates aspects of expressive writing and motivation interviewing into an interactive dialogue agent that adapts to participant writing behavior.In this study, we extended our previously developed system, Expressive Interviewing [
], to engage users to reflect on the pandemic, with the goal of reducing stress and encouraging positive behavior change. We recruited 151 participants through an online survey to test the effect of Expressive Interviewing on a variety of psychosocial and behavioral outcomes. Our target population for the study was people who were open to trying new forms of technology and who, during the early stages of the pandemic, had concerns about COVID-19 that they wanted to share in writing.We investigated the following research questions (RQs) with respect to Expressive Interviewing:
- RQ1: What are the short-term effects of Expressive Interviewing on an individual’s mental health?
- RQ2: What are the long-term effects (after 2 weeks) of Expressive Interviewing on an individual’s behavior?
- RQ3: How do the short-term and long-term effects vary based on Expressive Interviewing participants’ writing style and demographic characteristics?
Our study addressed these questions through quantitative analyses of the survey responses from task and control participants before and after the intervention.
To identify short-term and long-term effects of the intervention, we focused on the following dependent variables as possible areas of change: stress, COVID-19–related mental health, COVID-19 awareness, social gathering, social discussions, and general mental health. The mental health factors were chosen to align with previous research showing how automated dialogue agents can provide effective support for mental health concerns [
]. The social behavior factors and COVID-19–specific health factors were chosen to study the possible uncertainty around norms and rules that most US residents were facing at the time of the study (ie, mid-vaccine rollout). Considering the prior work that investigates the impact of chatbots on COVID-19–related issues such as vaccination [ ], we studied health outcomes that had a clear connection to both individual-level behaviors (eg, vaccination) and collective behaviors (eg, socialization).Methods
Expressive Interviewing System
The Expressive Interviewing system was built on principles of expressive writing and motivational interviewing, drawing from the values of both writing about personal experiences and empathetic communication through reflective listening [
, ]. A previous version of the system was described by Welch et al [ ]. We describe the system’s general functions in the following paragraphs and show an example interaction in .The system conducts an interview-style interaction with users about how the COVID-19 pandemic has been affecting them. The system’s goal is to encourage users to write as much as possible to explore their thoughts and feelings surrounding features of COVID-19. The interview consists of a set of writing prompts in the form of questions about specific issues related to the pandemic. This follows from prior research in expressive writing that shows the cathartic benefits of guided writing [
]. The system guides the interaction based on users’ responses, provides reflective feedback, and asks additional questions whenever appropriate. Although the system has a chat-like interface, participants are asked to write long responses, in contrast with a more open-domain chat setting without strong expectations for the human participant.In order to provide reflective feedback, the system automatically detects the topics being discussed (eg, work, family) or emotions being expressed (eg, anger, anxiety) and responds with reflections that ask participants to further expand upon their feelings, ask what they can do to help improve a situation for themselves or others, or ask how one can best cope with their feelings. These reflections are direct responses to the feelings or topics mentioned, making them prompts that ask the user to write more about the situation or what they can do to change it. For instance, if the system detects work as a topic of interest, it responds with “How has work changed under COVID? What might you be able to do to keep your career moving during these difficult times?” Empathy is expressed by showing an understanding of what the participant is saying. Reflections contain phrases acknowledging the participant’s emotion (eg, “There is sadness in your writing”) or the subject of concern (eg, “You mention issues related to money and finance”). After the interaction ends (ie, all prompts have been answered by the user), the system provides detailed visual and textual feedback. Reflection can express empathy and is often perceived as affirming.
Each conversation consists of a series of 4 main writing prompt questions. The prompts were iteratively designed in collaboration with experts in psychology, health communications, and public health with specialties in expressive writing and motivational interviewing. The prompts for the Expressive Interviewing system are described in the list that follows. The order of the latter 3 is not fixed (eg, some people saw the “looking forward” prompt after the “advice” prompt). Note that some prompts have undergone wording changes as compared with Welch et al [
] to reflect the ongoing state of the pandemic.- What are the major issues in your life right now, especially in the light of issues surrounding COVID-19?
- What is something you look forward to doing in the upcoming year?
- What advice would you give other people about how to cope with any of the issues you are facing?
- COVID-19 continues to affect our lives in many ways, but people have the amazing ability to find good things even in the most challenging situations. What is something that you have done or experienced recently that you are grateful for?
The system relies on rules rather than machine learning, which is a design choice motivated by the risks of deploying generative models, especially in regard to sensitive topics and mental health issues [
]. This also controls the dialog in such a way that participants have somewhat similar experiences, answering at least 4 of the same prompts (per topic). The full algorithm is provided in .After the interaction, users are shown graphical and templated textual feedback describing their word usage: how meaningful, how self-reflective, and how their emotional tone sounded. The system is hosted on a server belonging to our research lab and was easily accessible online during the study.
Experimental Design
To evaluate the system, we recruited 2 groups of participants from the crowdsource platform Prolific, and we randomly assigned them to the Expressive Interviewing and control (no Expressive Interviewing) conditions. All participants were prevented from participating in more than one condition to better isolate the effects of the Expressive Interviewing task. The participants were not told beforehand into which condition they would be placed, to reduce the chance of selection bias. The task participants and control participants completed the same presurvey, which included questions about the participant’s background as well as their behaviors relating to COVID-19 safety (eg, socializing in public) and general mental health (depression prevalence). The task participants were then immediately redirected to the Expressive Interviewing website afterward to begin their session. The task participants completed another short survey immediately before and after the task about their mental state, to address short-term effects.
Note that the control participants only completed a survey to track the long-term outcome variables (ie, no tracking of short-term outcome variables), unlike the Expressive Interviewing participants who completed surveys for both long-term and short-term outcome variables.
At 2 weeks after the initial presurvey, we sent an identical postsurvey to all participants in the Expressive Interviewing and control groups. The control participants were less likely to finish the experiment than the Expressive Interviewing participants, but all conditions had reasonable completion rates (63/100, 63% control; 88/100, 88% Expressive Interviewing).
Participants were compensated for their time in the study with a payment of US $4.50 for the pre-intervention survey and postintervention survey, under an assumption of 30 minutes per task and US $9 per hour as a reasonable wage. Participants in the Expressive Interviewing condition received a bonus payment of US $1.00 for completing the surveys and the treatment. We reminded participants that they needed to spend at least 15 minutes on their writing task and that their payment would be rejected if the system logged less than 15 minutes of writing.
We show a summary of the participant data, including demographics, in
.Participant statistics | Expressive Interviewing (n=88) | Control (n=63) | |||
Education, n (%) | |||||
Associate degree | 10 (11) | 6 (10) | |||
Bachelor’s degree | 29 (33) | 20 (32) | |||
High school | 24 (27) | 23 (37) | |||
Master’s degree | 22 (25) | 15 (24) | |||
PhD or higher | 2 (2) | 2 (3) | |||
Prefer not to say | 1 (1) | 1 (2) | |||
Some high school | 1 (1) | 1 (2) | |||
Ethnicity, n (%) | |||||
Asian or Asian American | 11 (13) | 7 (11) | |||
Black or African American | 14 (16) | 6 (10) | |||
Latinx or Hispanic | 4 (5) | 2 (3) | |||
Other | 2 (2) | 2 (3) | |||
White or Caucasian | 58 (66) | 51 (81) | |||
Gender, n (%) | |||||
Female | 36 (41) | 37 (59) | |||
Male | 48 (55) | 26 (41) | |||
Nonbinary | 3 (3) | 4 (6) | |||
Other | 2 (2) | 1 (2) | |||
Age (years), median | 32 | 29.5 | |||
Interview | |||||
Response length (words), mean | 53.3 | N/Aa | |||
Interview time (minutes), mean | 15.2 | N/A |
aN/A: not applicable.
Ethical Considerations
This study was approved by the institutional review board at the University of Michigan (HUM00182586). Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. We provided participants with the choice to opt out of the study when they opened the pre-intervention and postintervention survey. We provided the following guarantee of anonymity to participants before they begin interacting with the Expressive Interviewing system: “Everything you write will be kept completely confidential. In fact, since the interview is conducted by a computer program, you should feel free to be even more honest and direct than you might usually be. Note that this is meant to be a personal interview to learn more about your reactions to the pandemic. Hopefully, by answering these questions, you will learn more about your own reactions to the outbreak.” We did not ask for personal identifiers and data are stored anonymously. The Expressive Interviewing system and participant response data were stored on a secure server and were only accessed by the study authors. The website is secured with SSL following recent security recommendations. The conversation data cannot be de-anonymized due to the personal nature of the conversations and so will not be made publicly available.
Example System Output
In
, we show an example interaction of a study participant with the system. We highlight the adaptive nature of the Expressive Interviewing system: In response to the participant’s discussion of work conditions, the system asks a question about the effect of the pandemic on work in general (“How has work changed under COVID?”).Results
Aggregate Outcomes
Short-term Outcome Variables
Our study investigated the following short-term variables, which task participants were asked to provide immediately before or after completing Expressive Interviewing (all values scaled from 1 to 7):
- Life satisfaction (before): In general, how satisfied are you with your life?
- Stress (before/after): How stressed are you feeling right now?
- Meaningful (after): How valuable and meaningful was the interview to you?
- Personal (after): Overall, how personal was the interview to you?
We show the distribution of all the short-term outcome variables in
. Although most participants tended to report feeling highly satisfied with their life and happy with the experience, the Expressive Interviewing participants showed a significant decrease in reported stress after completing the task (absolute change –0.80, ~23% of the original value; see ).Outcome variable | Description | Results, mean (SD) |
Pretask stress | How stressed are you feeling right now? | 3.41 (1.82) |
Posttask stress | How stressed are you feeling right now? | 2.58 (1.57) |
Meaningful | How valuable and meaningful was the interview to you? | 5.32 (1.76) |
Personal | Overall, how personal was the interview to you? | 5.58 (1.58) |
Life satisfaction | In general, how satisfied are you with your life? | 4.72 (1.58) |
Outcome variables | Difference | F (df) | P value |
Stress | –0.80 | 19.48 (87) | <.001 |
Long-term Outcome Variables.
We aggregated the individual long-term outcome variables in our analysis (using a simple mean to aggregate multiple variables), based on whether they measure similar constructs (eg, different aspects of social behavior include going out to eat and meeting friends in public). The aggregated variables are explained in
, and the individual variables that are combined to form each group variable are also listed. The scale of each aggregate variable is 0 to 7 (number of days per week spent on a behavior).Outcome variable | Subcomponent variablesa | Results, mean (SD) |
COVID-19 mental behavior | COVID-control, COVID-hope, COVID-plan, COVID-sleep, COVID-worry (5) | 4.42 (1.24) |
COVID-19 awareness | COVID-recommend, COVID-talk, COVID-nervous-others, COVID-reading, COVID-watching (5) | 2.27 (1.79) |
Social activity | Social-face-to-face, Social-gathering, Social-public, Social-restaurant (4) | 1.74 (1.29) |
Social discussions | Discussions-digital, Discussions-message (2) | 4.02 (1.61) |
Mental health | Mental-changes, Mental-depressed, Mental-interest, Mental-nervous, Mental-worry (5) | 0.85 (0.77) |
aSee
for explanations of the subcomponent variables.To explain the long-term dependent variables, we provide the following example. The “COVID-control” subcomponent variable (corresponding to the COVID-19 mental behavior outcome) represents the participants’ responses to the question “In the last week, on how many days did you feel in control of your life and able to handle challenges that might come your way?” The participants could respond with a number between 0 and 7 to indicate the duration (in days) of the feeling of being in control and able to handle challenges.
We compared the long-term changes in the outcome variables from pre to posttask (after a 2-week gap) against the changes for the control participants. We performed an ANOVA for differences between the presurvey and postsurvey conditions (see
). We found a significant difference in COVID-19 awareness postsurvey compared with presurvey (mean difference=–0.267; F1=21.7, P<.001) and a significant difference in social activity between the Expressive Interviewing and control groups (mean difference=0.151; F1=4.75, P=.03). The decrease in COVID-19 awareness may be related to the decreasing importance of COVID-19–related protocols at the time of the survey (May 2021 and June 2021).Condition | Sum of squares | F (df) | P value | ||||
COVID-19 mental behavior | |||||||
Experiment condition | 1.227 | 0.807 (1) | .37 | ||||
Survey time | 0.262 | 0.172 (1) | .68 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey timea | 0.677 | 0.446 (3) | .505 | ||||
Residual | 743.131 | N/Ab | N/A | ||||
COVID-19 awareness | |||||||
Experiment condition | 8.791 | 3.165 (1) | .08 | ||||
Survey time | 60.380 | 21.742 (1) | <.001 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey time | 0.042 | 0.015 (3) | .90 | ||||
Residual | 1360.800 | N/A | N/A | ||||
Vaccination | |||||||
Experiment condition | 1.190 | 1.442 (1) | .23 | ||||
Survey time | 2.552 | 3.091 (1) | .08 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey time | 0.087 | 0.105 (3) | .75 | ||||
Residual | 403.628 | N/A | N/A | ||||
Mental health | |||||||
Experiment condition | 0.184 | 0.291 (1) | .59 | ||||
Survey time | 1.234 | 1.954 (1) | .16 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey time | 0.267 | 0.422 (3) | .52 | ||||
Residual | 308.861 | N/A | N/A | ||||
Social activity | |||||||
Experiment condition | 7.722 | 4.750 (1) | .03 | ||||
Survey time | 2.294 | 1.411 (1) | .24 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey time | 0.066 | 0.040 (3) | .84 | ||||
Residual | 794.938 | N/A | N/A | ||||
Social discussions | |||||||
Experiment condition | 0.008 | 0.003 (1) | .96 | ||||
Survey time | 4.000 | 1.624 (1) | .20 | ||||
Experiment condition * survey time | 0.002 | 0.001 (3) | .98 | ||||
Residual | 1204.258 | N/A | N/A |
aThe asterisk indicates interaction between different independent variables.
bN/A: not applicable.
Between-Group Comparisons
Some of the outcome variables of this study, such as mental health, are known to correlate with demographics [
], and it is also plausible that some participants’ backgrounds might have influenced their interaction with the Expressive Interviewing system (eg, prior vaccination status). Considering this, we therefore investigated whether the outcomes vary at all based on participant demographic or subgroup. We ran a separate ANOVA for each participant subgroup category, including gender, ethnicity, education level, age, and vaccination status (no vaccination, partial vaccination, and full vaccination).To compare subgroups between conditions, we used the following formula to fit the ANOVA model: Outcome variable ~ Participant subgroup + Survey time + Survey condition + Participant subgroup * Survey condition + Participant subgroup * Survey time
For brevity, we only report significant effects for the interaction between participant subgroup and survey condition (eg, whether male participants in the Expressive Interviewing condition had a different outcome than female participants in the Expressive Interviewing condition). We did not report significant effects for the participant subgroups on their own (eg, whether all male participants in the Expressive Interviewing condition and the control condition experienced a change in outcome).
We found significant effects for participant subgroups on outcome variables, after correcting for multiple comparisons: P=.05/(5 participant subgroups * 7 outcome variables)=0.00143. See
.For the one significant interaction effect that we did find (social activity outcome ~ ethnicity * condition), the differences between subgroups were as follows. In the Expressive Interviewing condition, African American participants had a higher rate of social activity than participants of a different ethnicity (Caucasian: U=4490, P<.001; Asian American: U=596; P<.001). In the control condition, Asian American participants had a higher rate of social activity than African American participants (U=114, P=.045), Caucasian participants (U=373, P=.004), and Hispanic participants (U=47.5, P=.04).
Most of the differences are not relevant for our study, because there were differences between the aforementioned subgroups but not between conditions (ie, not between Expressive Interviewing and control groups). For example, male participants did not show a significant difference in COVID-19 mental behavior between the Expressive Interviewing and control conditions, even though we found a significant difference based on gender for COVID-19 mental behavior in general.
We report all median values for the outcome variables for the aforementioned subgroups in
.Outcome variables and participant subgroups | F (df) | P value | |
COVID-19 mental behavior | |||
Gender | 17.9 (1) | <.001 | |
COVID-19 awareness | |||
Gender | 15.5 (1) | <.001 | |
Vaccination | |||
Ethnicity | 15.5 (4) | <.001 | |
Education | 7.06 (6) | <.001 | |
Gender | 11.3 (1) | <.001 | |
Mental health | |||
Age | 14.8 (2) | <.001 | |
Education | 8.75 (6) | <.001 | |
Gender | 23.8 (1) | <.001 | |
Social activity | |||
Ethnicity | 6.18 (4) | <.001 | |
Ethnicity * condition | 5.93 (9) | <.001 | |
Education | 7.10 (6) | <.001 | |
Vaccination | 12.7 (2) | <.001 | |
Socialdiscussions | |||
Education | 12.2 (6) | <.001 | |
Stress | |||
Age | 24.4 (2) | <.001 |
Ethnicity | Control, median | Expressive Interviewing, median |
African American | 1.5 | 2.5 |
Asian American | 1 | 2.375 |
Caucasian | 1.125 | 1.5 |
Hispanic | 1 | 1.375 |
Other | 0.5 | 1.25 |
Correlation With Writing Behavior
Having analyzed the aggregate and subgroup trends in outcome variables, we next analyzed the participants’ writing behavior within the Expressive Interviewing condition. We focused on the correlations with both short-term effects and long-term effects. To quantify linguistic patterns, we leveraged several word categories from Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) [
] to characterize participant behavior through their interview responses (eg, the 1-word category contains “anxiety”-related words such as “nervous,” which we find highly useful in understanding participant responses to COVID-19–related problems).Short-term Effects
As described in the previous sections, participants demonstrated a consistent decrease in self-reported stress immediately after finishing the task (see
). We then investigated whether that decrease is correlated with particular language patterns. We correlated all participants’ interview language variables with their change in self-reported stress, presurvey life satisfaction, and postsurvey rating of the task as meaningful and personal. Some of these interview variables showed possible outlier effects (eg, 1 participant used more than twice as many ANGER words as the next-highest participant). We tried to limit these outlier effects by first removing all participants with interview response variables above the 95th percentile, then computing the correlation for each response variable on the filtered data.We list all significant correlations in
. A few findings are highlighted here. Using more anxiety words and fewer COVID-19 words was correlated with a decrease in reported stress. This point is particularly pertinent in the context of COVID-19 because people have reported feeling higher levels of anxiety due to social isolation [ ]. Using more COVID-19 words and more positive emotion words was correlated with a more personal and more meaningful experience. Participants who reported higher life satisfaction before the session also used fewer anxiety words and fewer sad words in their responses.Response variable | Outcome variable | r | P value |
FEAR | Pretask stress | 0.216 | .003 |
POSITIVE EMOTION | Pretask stress | –0.204 | .005 |
ANXa | Stress change | –0.264 | .006 |
COVIDb | Meaningful | 0.259 | <.001 |
POSITIVE EMOTION | Meaningful | 0.243 | .001 |
COVIDa | Personal | 0.214 | .003 |
JOY | Personal | 0.242 | .001 |
POSITIVE EMOTION | Personal | 0.242 | .001 |
Prompt response overlap | Personal | 0.196 | .007 |
ANX | Life satisfaction | –0.210 | .004 |
SAD | Life satisfaction | –0.207 | .004 |
aAnxiety words.
bThe “COVID” variable includes words related to COVID-19, such as “coronavirus.”
To better understand the connection between anxiety and stress, we investigated a few sample interview messages that contained high rates of anxiety words written by participants who reported a decrease in stress after the session. Some people discussed current events and their anxious feelings around them (eg, “This is just so scary when I see on the news that people are targeting Asians.”). Other participants mentioned general mental health struggles (eg, “I’m worried about becoming destitute, lonely, and depressed as I grow older and elder members of my family die.”). However, a considerable number of participants also framed anxiety in a positive light: In response to a question about advice for others dealing with COVID-19–related problems, a participant wrote “Learn a new skill, do something proactive, even volunteering would help cope with stress and everyday struggles.”
Long-term Effects
To address the possible effects of the interview itself, we compared the changes in outcome variables to the language choices by the task participants. We computed the Pearson correlation between the interview variables and the long-term variables. We found the following significant correlations (reported in
): (1) Task participants who used a diverse vocabulary had an increase in COVID-19 awareness, (2) task participants who used a high amount of negative emotion words had an increase in mental health (ie, worse negative health), and (3) task participants who wrote longer responses and had a less diverse vocabulary also had a decrease in social activity.Response variable | Outcome variable | r | P value |
Lexical diversity | COVID-19 awareness | 0.226 | .003 |
NEGATIVE EMOTION | Mental health | 0.229 | .002 |
Length | Social activity | –0.276 | <.001 |
Lexical diversity | Social activity | 0.266 | <.001 |
Discussion
Principal Findings
This study investigated the potential psychosocial and behavioral impact of Expressive Interviewing in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. We investigated short-term patterns such as stress (RQ1), for long-term patterns such as social behavior (RQ2), and for between-participant patterns that included demographics and writing style (RQ3).
Our study shows that Expressive Interviewing participants (ie, treatment only) generally exhibited a short-term decrease in stress after the task. This correlation was particularly strong for participants who expressed more anxiety during their interaction with the system. As compared with the control condition, the task participants in general did not show significant long-term changes in COVID-19–related behavior, but some subpopulations of the participants did show changes regardless of condition. We found a slight difference in social activity between the Expressive Interviewing and control participants.
Our study suggests that Expressive Interviewing may help participants handle short-term stress related to COVID-19 and that people who write differently via the system may experience different long-term outcomes.
We expected some amount of change among all participants, considering that the Expressive Interviewing prompts ask users to reflect on their own mental behavior and their prospects for the future. In general, the participants who received Expressive Interviewing as “treatment” did not experience a consistent change in long-term COVID-19–related behavior or mental outcomes. However, we also saw a surprising amount of variance in results among task participants based on the content of their responses (eg, participants who wrote with more anxiety words also experienced a greater decrease in stress).
We interpret the findings as a useful example of the intended scope of chat-based therapy. As prior work suggests [
], we should not expect automated dialogue agents to address serious mental health problems but instead consider that they may be best suited for surface-level issues. A short-term decrease in stress is certainly nothing to dismiss, but such a change should not be considered a “fix” for a deep-seated problem such as social anxiety with respect to COVID-19–related policies [ ]. Furthermore, the variation in responses based on writing style corroborates other studies with automated dialogue agents (eg, different levels of dropout based on perceived self-efficacy in therapy with a chatbot [ ]). We should anticipate that people will react very differently to the same dialogue system given how disparately the pandemic has impacted different subpopulations [ ].Limitations
The findings of this study should be taken in the context of the time in which the study was conducted. In June 2021, the United States had opened COVID-19 vaccinations to most of the adult population, and many previous restrictions about public gatherings were being lifted. This time period marked a significant change in the popular perception of the pandemic from an unbeatable disease to a problem that seemed to be controllable through vaccination [
]. The participants in our study may have considered COVID-19 to be a “solved” problem and may not have experienced a long-term reaction to the task in the same way that they would have in the earlier stage of the pandemic. Some of the variables studied should also be considered in the context of the time, such as vaccination status. Although, by June 2021, COVID-19 vaccinations were technically available to much of the public, many groups still faced difficulty or expressed skepticism at the technology due to its speed of development and the unknown efficacy in the long term [ ]. Therefore, unvaccinated individuals in our study may have had multiple reasons for behaving differently after the task, which are hard to assess without further interviewing participants.We also note that the participants recruited through Prolific may not be identical to the general population [
], raising the possibility of selection bias. In particular, crowd workers may have different values and attitudes than the general population [ ], as well as different demographics due to different socioeconomic statuses among minority populations [ ]. The constraints of the task may have also biased the participants’ responses, especially the requirement for a minimum amount of time spent on each response during the Expressive Interviewing task (for example, time constraints among crowd workers [ ]). We do not claim that our results will generalize to the general public but instead view this study as an initial inquiry into the value of automated dialogue systems for helping with self-reflection through guided writing [ ].The political situation should also be emphasized. By June 2021, the issue of COVID-19 in the United States had become highly polarized [
], to the point where people were unlikely to be persuaded to change their behavior. This could explain the divergent responses in long-term outcomes among different subpopulations of participants (eg, increase in social activity among no-vaccine participants, who by this point had already made up their mind about their own COVID-19–related behavior). Polarization in COVID-19 attitudes may explain the importance of the interview variables (eg, anger words) in explaining differences in long-term outcomes. Participants with strong attitudes toward COVID-19 may have used Expressive Interviewing to better explore those attitudes and therefore experienced a stronger long-term change, indicated by the fact that participants who used many negative emotion words also experienced an increase in COVID-19 mental health outcomes. Further work in this direction should consider political ideology as a possible social dimension that can affect responses to Expressive Interviewing, as prior work has found that political ideology can affect responses to behavior change interventions [ ].Conclusion
Expressive Interviewing can help people struggling with difficult life situations to navigate their mental and social health. This study focused primarily on addressing health behavior change with respect to COVID-19, but the analysis could be readily extended to other topical domains in which self-reflection could lead to behavior change. Our system provides a helpful starting point for future research, and we encourage researchers to modify the existing prompts for our study to match future social situations (eg, addressing anti-Asian hate in the wake of COVID-19 [
]). The findings of this study can also inform mental health counselors who want to use expressive writing to encourage behavior change in dynamic settings such as COVID-19. Although we found that a single intervention of Expressive Interviewing does not correspond to long-term change, future work may find that a series of interventions could improve patient outcomes by reinforcement [ ]. By developing writing prompts that match the setting and the patient’s background, a counselor may be able to engage the patient more effectively.Acknowledgments
This material is based in part on work supported by the Precision Health initiative at the University of Michigan and the John Templeton Foundation (grant #61156). Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Precision Health initiative or the John Templeton Foundation.
Data Availability
We cannot share the conversation data collected during this study due to sensitive information that participants shared during their conversations (eg, specific medical problems exacerbated by COVID-19). The de-identified survey data analyzed in the study are available from the corresponding author for research purposes only, by request.
Conflicts of Interest
JP is the owner of Pennebaker Conglomerates Inc, the company that owns the academic version of Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC). All profits from LIWC sales are donated to educational and charitable groups.
Editorial Notice
This randomized study was only retrospectively registered, as authors were unaware of the requirement for pre-registration of clinical trials. The editor granted an exception from ICMJE rules mandating prospective registration of randomized trials because the risk of bias appears low and the study was considered formative. However, readers are advised to carefully assess the validity of any potential explicit or implicit claims related to primary outcomes or effectiveness, as retrospective registration does not prevent authors from changing their outcome measures retrospectively.
Descriptive statistics about independent and dependent variables, description of independent linguistic variables, and additional regression analysis of results.
DOCX File , 37 KBCONSORT-eHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 296 KBReferences
- Green J, Huberty J, Puzia M, Stecher C. The effect of meditation and physical activity on the mental health impact of COVID-19-related stress and attention to news among mobile app users in the United States: cross-sectional survey. JMIR Ment Health. Apr 13, 2021;8(4):e28479. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Han L, Zhan Y, Li W, Xu Y, Xu Y, Zhao J. Associations between the perceived severity of the COVID-19 pandemic, cyberchondria, depression, anxiety, stress, and lockdown experience: cross-sectional survey study. JMIR Public Health Surveill. Sep 16, 2021;7(9):e31052. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Frattaroli J. Experimental disclosure and its moderators: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. Nov 2006;132(6):823-865. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Reinhold M, Bürkner P, Holling H. Effects of expressive writing on depressive symptoms — A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. Mar 2018;25(1):1. [CrossRef]
- Small L, Anderson D, Sidora-Arcoleo K, Gance-Cleveland B. Pediatric nurse practitioners' assessment and management of childhood overweight/obesity: results from 1999 and 2005 cohort surveys. J Pediatr Health Care. 2009;23(4):231-241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Brodie DA, Inoue A, Shaw DG. Motivational interviewing to change quality of life for people with chronic heart failure: a randomised controlled trial. Int J Nurs Stud. Apr 2008;45(4):489-500. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- D'Amico EJ, Miles JN, Stern SA, Meredith LS. Brief motivational interviewing for teens at risk of substance use consequences: a randomized pilot study in a primary care clinic. J Subst Abuse Treat. Jul 2008;35(1):53-61. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Friederichs S, Bolman C, Oenema A, Guyaux J, Lechner L. Motivational interviewing in a Web-based physical activity intervention with an avatar: randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. Feb 13, 2014;16(2):e48. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Mitchell S, Heyden R, Heyden N, Schroy P, Andrew S, Sadikova E, et al. A pilot study of motivational interviewing training in a virtual world. J Med Internet Res. Sep 26, 2011;13(3):e77. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Leong FTL, Kalibatseva Z. Cross-cultural barriers to mental health services in the United States. Cerebrum. Mar 2011;2011:5. [FREE Full text] [Medline]
- Saraceno B, van Ommeren M, Batniji R, Cohen A, Gureje O, Mahoney J, et al. Barriers to improvement of mental health services in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. Sep 29, 2007;370(9593):1164-1174. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Golberstein E, Wen H, Miller BF. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) and mental health for children and adolescents. JAMA Pediatr. Sep 01, 2020;174(9):819-820. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Miner AS, Milstein A, Hancock JT. Talking to machines about personal mental health problems. JAMA. Oct 03, 2017;318(13):1217-1218. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Vaidyam AN, Wisniewski H, Halamka JD, Kashavan MS, Torous JB. Chatbots and conversational agents in mental health: a review of the psychiatric landscape. Can J Psychiatry. Jul 2019;64(7):456-464. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Boucher EM, Harake NR, Ward HE, Stoeckl SE, Vargas J, Minkel J, et al. Artificially intelligent chatbots in digital mental health interventions: a review. Expert Rev Med Devices. Dec 2021;18(sup1):37-49. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Welch C, Lahnala A, Perez-Rosas V, Shen S, Seraj S, An L, et al. Expressive Interviewing: A Conversational System for Coping with COVID-19. Presented at: 1st Workshop on NLP for COVID-19 (Part 2) at EMNLP 2020; December 2020, 2020; Online. URL: https://aclanthology.org/2020.nlpcovid19-2.6/ [CrossRef]
- Luk TT, Lui JHT, Wang MP. Efficacy, usability, and acceptability of a chatbot for promoting COVID-19 vaccination in unvaccinated or booster-hesitant young adults: pre-post pilot study. J Med Internet Res. Oct 04, 2022;24(10):e39063. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational Interviewing: Helping People Change, 3rd Edition. New York, NY. The Guilford Press; 2013.
- Pennebaker JW, Chung CK. Expressive writing: Connections to physical and mental health. In: Friedman HS, editor. The Oxford handbook of health psychology. Oxford, England. Oxford University Press; 2014;417-437.
- Henderson P, Sinha K, Angelard-Gontier N, Ke NR, Fried G, Lowe R, et al. Ethical Challenges in Data-Driven Dialogue Systems. Presented at: AAAI/ACM Conference on AI, Ethics, and Society; February 2-3, 2018, 2018; New Orleans, LA. [CrossRef]
- Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0). Creative Commons. URL: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ [accessed 2023-05-06]
- Lascau L, Gould SJJ, Cox AL, Karmannaya E, Brumby DP. Monotasking or Multitasking: Designing for Crowdworkers' Preferences. Presented at: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; May 4-9, 2019, 2019; Glasgow, Scotland. [CrossRef]
- Posch L, Bleier A, Flöck F, Lechner CM, Kinder-Kurlanda K, Helic D, et al. Characterizing the global crowd workforce: a cross-country comparison of crowdworker demographics. Human Computation. Aug 03, 2022;9(1):22-57. [CrossRef]
- Pennebaker JW, Francis ME, Booth RJ. Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC2001. LIWC. Mahwah, NJ. Erlbaum Publishers; 2001. URL: https://www.liwc.app/static/documents/LIWC2001%20Manual%20-%20Operation%2C%20Development%2C%20and%20Psychometrics.pdf [accessed 2023-04-24]
- Banerjee D, Rai M. Social isolation in Covid-19: The impact of loneliness. Int J Soc Psychiatry. Sep 2020;66(6):525-527. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Xiong J, Lipsitz O, Nasri F, Lui LMW, Gill H, Phan L, et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. Dec 01, 2020;277:55-64. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Perrone M, Durbin DA. COVID-19 surge in the US: The summer of hope ends in gloom. AP News. Sep 08, 2021. URL: https://apnews.com/article/lifestyle-health-pandemics-coronavirus-pandemic-north-carolina-b9012bd212ef28dcceec583eb3ca7612 [accessed 2022-12-01]
- Machingaidze S, Wiysonge CS. Understanding COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Nat Med. Aug 2021;27(8):1338-1339. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Turner AM, Engelsma T, Taylor JO, Sharma RK, Demiris G. Recruiting older adult participants through crowdsourcing platforms: Mechanical Turk versus Prolific Academic. AMIA Annu Symp Proc. 2020;2020:1230-1238. [FREE Full text] [Medline]
- Deng X, Joshi KD. Why individuals participate in micro-task crowdsourcing work environment: revealing crowdworkers’ perceptions. Journal of the Association for Information Systems. Nov 2016;17(10):648-673. [CrossRef]
- Druckman JN, Klar S, Krupnikov Y, Levendusky M, Ryan JB. Affective polarization, local contexts and public opinion in America. Nat Hum Behav. Jan 2021;5(1):28-38. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bail CA, Argyle LP, Brown TW, Bumpus JP, Chen H, Hunzaker MBF, et al. Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase political polarization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Sep 11, 2018;115(37):9216-9221. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gover AR, Harper SB, Langton L. Anti-Asian hate crime during the COVID-19 pandemic: exploring the reproduction of inequality. Am J Crim Justice. Jul 07, 2020;45(4):647-667. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bundy C. Changing behaviour: using motivational interviewing techniques. J R Soc Med. 2004;97 Suppl 44(Suppl 44):43-47. [FREE Full text] [Medline]
Abbreviations
LIWC: Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count |
RQ: research question |
Edited by A Mavragani; submitted 13.06.22; peer-reviewed by J Gamble; comments to author 19.12.22; revised version received 30.03.23; accepted 30.03.23; published 01.08.23.
Copyright©Ian Stewart, Charles Welch, Lawrence An, Ken Resnicow, James Pennebaker, Rada Mihalcea. Originally published in JMIR Formative Research (https://formative.jmir.org), 01.08.2023.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Formative Research, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://formative.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.